Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Journal of Pediatrics 1/2024

Open Access 06.11.2023 | RESEARCH

Predicting long-term neurocognitive outcome after pediatric intensive care unit admission for bronchiolitis—preliminary exploration of the potential of machine learning

verfasst von: Eleonore S. V. de Sonnaville, Jacob Vermeule, Kjeld Oostra, Hennie Knoester, Job B. M. van Woensel, Somaya Ben Allouch, Jaap Oosterlaan, Marsh Kӧnigs

Erschienen in: European Journal of Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2024

Abstract

Purpose

For successful prevention and intervention, it is important to unravel the complex constellation of factors that affect neurocognitive functioning after pediatric intensive care unit (PICU) admission. This study aims (1) to elucidate the potential relevance of patient and PICU-related characteristics for long-term adverse neurocognitive outcome after PICU admission for bronchiolitis, and (2) to perform a preliminary exploration of the potential of machine learning as compared to linear regression to improve neurocognitive outcome prediction in a relatively small sample of children after PICU admission.

Methods

This cross-sectional observational study investigated 65 children aged 6–12 years with previous PICU admission for bronchiolitis (age ≤ 1 year). They were compared to demographically comparable healthy peers (n = 76) on neurocognitive functioning. Patient and PICU-related characteristics used for the prediction models were as follows: demographic characteristics, perinatal and disease parameters, laboratory results, and intervention characteristics, including hourly validated mechanical ventilation parameters. Neurocognitive outcome was measured by intelligence and computerized neurocognitive testing. Prediction models were developed for each of the neurocognitive outcomes using Regression Trees, k-Nearest Neighbors, and conventional linear regression analysis.

Results

The patient group had lower intelligence than the control group (p < .001, d = −0.59) and poorer performance in neurocognitive functions, i.e., speed and attention (p = .03, d = −0.41) and verbal memory (p < .001, d = −0.60). Lower intelligence was predicted by lower birth weight and lower socioeconomic status (R2 = 25.9%). Poorer performance on the speed and attention domain was predicted by younger age at follow-up (R2 = 53.5%). Poorer verbal memory was predicted by lower birth weight, younger age at follow-up, and greater exposure to acidotic events (R2 = 50.6%). The machine learning models did not reveal added value in terms of model performance as compared to linear regression.
Conclusion: The findings of this study suggest that in children with previous PICU admission for bronchiolitis, (1) lower birth weight, younger age at follow-up, and lower socioeconomic status are associated with poorer neurocognitive outcome; and (2) greater exposure to acidotic events during PICU admission is associated with poorer verbal memory outcome. The findings of this study provide no evidence for the added value of machine learning models as compared to linear regression analysis in the prediction of long-term neurocognitive outcome in a relatively small sample of children.
What is Known:
• Adverse neurocognitive outcomes are described in PICU survivors, which are known to interfere with development in other major domains of functioning, such as mental health, academic achievement, and socioeconomic success, highlighting neurocognition as an important outcome after PICU admission.
• Machine learning is a rapidly growing field of artificial intelligence that is increasingly applied in health care settings, with great potential to capture the complexity of outcome prediction.
What is New:
• This study shows that lower birth weight, lower socioeconomic status, and greater exposure to acidotic events during PICU admission for bronchiolitis are associated with poorer long-term neurocognitive outcome after PICU admission. Results provide no evidence for the added value of machine learning models in a relatively small sample of children.
• As bronchiolitis seldom manifests neurologically, the relation between acidotic events and neurocognitive outcome may reflect either potentially harmful effects of acidosis itself or related processes such as hypercapnia or hypoxic and/or ischemic events during PICU admission. This study further highlights the importance of structured follow-up to monitor long-term outcome of children after PICU admission.
Hinweise
Communicated by Piet Leroy

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00431-023-05307-3.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
CPR
Cardiopulmonary resuscitation
ECMO
Extracorporeal membrane oxygenation
etCO2
End-tidal carbon dioxide
FiO2
Fraction of inspired oxygen
FSIQ
Full-scale intelligence quotient
PEEP
Positive end-expiratory pressure
PICU
Pediatric intensive care unit
PIM2 score
Pediatric Index of Mortality 2 score
PIP
Positive inspiratory pressure
SpO2
Oxygen saturation

Introduction

With advances in pediatric intensive care, the survival rate of children admitted to the pediatric intensive care unit (PICU) has increased dramatically in the past decades [1, 2]. Yet, long-term morbidity after PICU admission is a growing concern [3, 4]. Sequelae are described in physical, neurocognitive, and psychosocial health [37]. Adverse neurocognitive outcomes are known to interfere with development in other major domains of functioning, such as physical and mental health [8, 9], academic achievement [10], and socioeconomic success (as measured by education, occupation, and income) [11], highlighting neurocognition as an important outcome after PICU admission.
In the literature, multiple pathophysiological mechanisms have been proposed that may contribute to long-term neurocognitive outcome of critically ill patients, including hypoxia, metabolic derangements such as glucose dysregulation and ischemia [1214]. Such mechanisms may be triggered by the underlying disease [15], the critical deterioration [7, 16], and/or the associated treatments at the PICU [17, 18]. In addition, also demographic characteristics such as age at PICU admission, age at follow-up, sex, and socioeconomic status have been found related to neurocognitive outcome after PICU admission [7, 1820]. As understanding of the origin of difficulties in neurocognitive functioning is a prerequisite for successful prevention and intervention, it is important to unravel the factors that affect neurocognitive functioning after PICU admission.
Digitalization of health care provides increasingly more data that can importantly contribute to better prediction and understanding of long-term outcome after PICU admission. Nevertheless, the increasing wealth of clinical data produced by medical devices involves very long time series representing a great number of characteristics with potential complex inter-relations that are relevant for outcome. Therefore, novel data sources challenge conventional statistical methods, such as linear regression, which are not suitable to handle larger numbers of predictors and have limited potential to capture complex relations between predictors and outcome. Compared to conventional statistics, machine learning has great potential to capture this complexity thanks to the capability to process vast amounts of data and model non-linear and highly complex interactions [21]. Machine learning is a rapidly growing field of artificial intelligence that is increasingly applied in health care settings [2225]. Given the large number of factors and mechanisms that have been implicated on the long-term neurocognitive outcome of critically ill patients, machine learning may have added value compared to linear regression to improve neurocognitive outcome prediction. However, the value of machine learning in investigating the relation between PICU admission and long-term neurocognitive outcome has not been investigated thus far and is therefore currently unclear.
This study aims (1) to elucidate the potential relevance of patient and PICU-related characteristics for long-term adverse neurocognitive outcome after PICU admission for bronchiolitis, and (2) to perform a preliminary exploration of the potential of machine learning as compared to linear regression to improve neurocognitive outcome prediction in a relatively small sample of children after PICU admission.

Materials and methods

Participants

This cross-sectional observational study assessed children aged 6–12 years with a history of PICU admission during infancy (age ≤ 1 year) for bronchiolitis requiring invasive mechanical ventilation (“patient group”). The diagnosis of bronchiolitis was defined as clinical symptoms of a lower respiratory tract infection including a positive viral nasopharyngeal aspirate. The patient group was compared to normally developing peers who had not been admitted to the PICU during their life (“control group”) on neurocognitive functioning. All participants were required to be proficient in the Dutch language. We included children from the age of six, as by that age the full range of neurocognitive functions can be assessed. We focused on children of primary school age, in an attempt to limit the heterogeneity in the development of the children. Exclusion criteria were as follows: developmental disorders known to impact on neurocognitive development; physical conditions and/or behavioral deficits interfering with the ability to adequately perform neurocognitive testing; clinical signs of neurological complications during PICU admission (e.g., seizure, encephalitis, meningitis); presence of family conflict interfering with study participation (e.g., child abuse, child being placed under external supervision); and living abroad. We specifically focused on children with previous PICU admission for bronchiolitis, because this is a relative homogenous group with single organ failure that seldom manifests neurologically [26, 27] and is therefore not expected to affect neurocognitive functioning in itself.
The patient group was retrospectively recruited from a consecutive cohort admitted between 2007 and 2013 to the PICU of the Amsterdam University Medical Centers (UMC), The Netherlands. All children in the patient group received similar treatment per local clinical protocol at time of PICU admission, including invasive mechanical ventilation modes, primary and secondary choice of sedative drugs during mechanical ventilation, oxygen therapy, and nutrition. The control group was recruited through the patient group (friends and relatives) and through primary schools in The Netherlands. We aimed to include at least 64 children in the patient group and 64 children in the control group, in order to achieve sufficient statistical power to detect medium-sized group differences (Cohen’s d = 0.5, assuming power = 80% and alpha = .05).

Measures

Data on socioeconomic status, past breastfeeding, mother’s smoking, and drinking of alcohol during pregnancy were collected using a parental questionnaire, as these characteristics have potential influence on children’s neurocognitive functioning [2830]. Socioeconomic status was defined as the average level of education for the available parent(s) and/or caregiver(s) and measured with the Education Categorization Standard developed by the Statistics Netherlands [31]. This standard assesses parental education on an eight-point interval scale ranging from 1 (no education) to 8 (postdoctoral education). In case patients only had one parent, socioeconomic status was determined by the level of parental education from only that parent. Furthermore, we extracted the following patient and PICU-related characteristics from the medical files (paper files and/ or electronic clinical information system MetaVision iMDsoft): sex, age, gestational age, birth weight, age and weight at PICU admission, Pediatric Index of Mortality 2 (PIM 2) score [32], duration of invasive mechanical ventilation, length of PICU stay, need for reintubation, cardiopulmonary resuscitation, use of antibiotics during PICU stay, readmission to the PICU, and the isolation of type(s) of viral agents from the nasopharyngeal aspirate. In case gestational age and birth weight had not been recorded in the medical file of the Amsterdam UMC, this information was requested from the hospital where the child was born. In addition, mechanical ventilation parameters provide information about the disease severity of the children and may therefore be relevant for their neurocognitive outcome, and also mechanical ventilation itself is potent to induce injury (e.g., to the lungs [33]). Therefore, we extracted the data of the mechanical ventilator that were hourly validated by the nurse: fraction of inspired oxygen (FiO2), positive inspiratory pressure (PIP), positive end-expiratory pressure (PEEP), mean airway pressure, oxygen saturation (SpO2), end-tidal carbon dioxide (etCO2), and SpO2/FiO2 ratio. At last, we extracted the laboratory measures of serum glucose, pH, partial pressure of carbon dioxide (pCO2), and lactate. Arterial and/ or capillary (in case patients did not have an arterial line) measures were extracted. In case patients were readmitted to the PICU, the PICU-related characteristics were collected from all PICU admissions together. After extraction of all characteristics (“raw data”), clinically relevant values were calculated (e.g., mean value, values below or above clinical cut-offs) [34]. Online Resource 1 displays the clinically relevant values of all extracted patient and PICU-related characteristics.

Long-term neurocognitive functioning

Neurocognitive functioning was assessed in our previous study [35] and was determined by assessment of full-scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) and specific domains of neurocognitive functioning by a standardized and computerized neurocognitive test-battery. FSIQ was assessed to capture general neurocognitive functioning and was measured by a short form of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third edition (WISC-III) involving two subtests measuring verbal IQ and two scales measuring performance IQ, i.e., the subtests Vocabulary, Arithmetic, Block Design, and Picture Arrangement. FSIQ estimated with this short form has excellent validity (r = .95) and reliability (r = .90) in the normative population as well as in a mixed neurological population (r = .86 and r = .96, respectively) [36, 37]. The neurocognitive test-battery measures a broad range of key neurocognitive domains and contains a composition of child-friendly tests based on well-known neuroscientific paradigms with established validity and reliability, i.e., the Attention Network Test [38], Multisensory Integration Task [39], Tower of London [40], Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test [41], Digit Span task [42], Klingberg task [43], and the Track & Trace task [44]. For more information, see Online Resource 2. The neurocognitive data derived from the test-battery were subjected to a pre-processing pipeline to construct neurocognitive domain scores [35]. This procedure resulted in ten neurocognitive domains that explained 78% of the variance contained in the original neurocognitive data derived from the test-battery, i.e., speed and attention, set shifting, verbal memory, visuomotor integration, verbal working memory, interference control, visual processing speed, visual working memory, planning time, and multisensory integration. Higher scores on each of the domains reflect better performance.

Procedure

Participating children underwent neurocognitive testing by trained examiners in a quiet room with an approximate duration of 3 h, including breaks. Block randomized order of test administration was applied to counterbalance the systematic influence of fatigue on test performance.

Pre-processing of patient and PICU-related characteristics

Missing values at random (< 10% missing values per variable) were imputed using multiple imputations [45]. Outliers (mean ± 3 SD) were winsorized [46, 47]. In order to avoid that the final model would be overly sensitive to variables with low prevalence, variables with fewer than 10 occurrences per event were eliminated. In the case of multicollinearity between variables (based on variance inflation factor > 10 and/or Pearson > 0.7 or < −0.7), the variable with the lowest correlation to FSIQ was eliminated.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using R [48], RStudio [49], the car package [50], and the caret package [51]. In order to gain insight in the association between predictor variables (patient and PICU-related characteristics) and long-term neurocognitive outcome, we selected two widely adopted machine learning algorithms Regression Trees and k-Nearest Neighbors that provide interpretable outcomes. We used multivariable linear regression analysis with backward elimination as a reference model (p out > 0.05). With each of the techniques (i.e., Regression Trees, k-Nearest Neighbors, Linear Regression), one model was fitted for each of the neurocognitive outcomes.
The goal of machine learning is to predict an outcome based on patterns present in the input data (training). In order to train a model to predict unseen (“new”) data, the original dataset was split into a training set (90% of the data) and a blind test set (10% of the data), which were identical for each model. The training set was then further divided into ten (folds) for five-repeated ten-fold cross-validation [52], which was used for performance validation. Each model was trained on data of the training set (nine out of ten folds), validating training performance on the tenth fold. Based on the results from model training, the mean performance across all folds was reported. Thereafter, the blind test set was used only once for each model, in order to assess internal model generalizability and model performance on data that were not used for model training. Internal model generalizability (i.e., stability of model performance on data that were not used to develop the model) was assessed by comparing model performance (the explained variance, R2) in the training set (average across folds) to the blind test set using 95% bootstrap confidence intervals (95%-CI). In case the mean R2 of the training set was within the 95%-CI of the R2 of the blind test set, we concluded that the model had sufficient internal generalization from the training data to the blind test data. Subsequently, model performance was based on the R2 in the blind test set. To assess the added value of the machine learning models as compared to our reference model, we compared the R2 of the blind test set between models, using the 95%-CI of the multivariable linear regression models as reference. For details regarding the machine learning algorithms, see Online Resource 3. All statistical testing was two-sided, α was set at .05.

Results

Participants

Children included in the patient group (n = 65, Fig. 1) did not differ from the total recruitment cohort of children satisfying the inclusion criteria (n = 119) in terms of sex, age at PICU admission, duration of mechanical ventilation, and length of PICU stay (Online Resource 4). In addition, comparison between children included in the patient group (n = 65) versus those eligible but not included (n = 54) also showed no significant differences regarding these characteristics (Online Resource 5), indicating no evidence for selection bias in the study sample. No differences between the patient and control group (n = 76) were found regarding sex, age, and socioeconomic status (Online Resource 6), indicating no evidence for a confounding role of demographic differences between groups. The study sample consists out of only unique patients, of which two had two PICU admissions for bronchiolitis, and five children were readmitted because of subglottic stenosis due to upper airway injury by endotracheal intubation. For these children, post-PICU time was used from the first PICU stay and PICU-related characteristics were based on all admissions together. Table 1 shows the patient and PICU-related characteristics of the included children that were used for the prediction models. Characteristics with an asterisk were eliminated in the Linear Regression and k-Nearest Neighbors models due to multicollinearity. Characteristics with less than ten occurrences per event were eliminated in all models and are only shown in Online Resource 1.
Table 1
Patient and PICU-related characteristics that were used for the prediction models
Patient and PICU-related characteristics
Mean (SD), median (IQR), or number (%)
Age at follow-up (years), mean (SD)
8.1 (1.2)
Sex (female), n (%)
26 (40.0)
Socioeconomic status, mean (SD)
5.3 (1.2)
Gestational age (weeks), median (IQR)a
38.1 (36.3–39.9)
Birth weight (grams), mean (SD)
3083 (968)
Breastfed in past, n (%)
42 (64.6)
Age at PICU admission (days), median (IQR)a
43.0 (23.5–79.5)
Weight at PICU admission (grams), mean (SD)
4634 (1662)
PIM 2 score, median (IQR)
1.4 (1.1–2.1)
Duration of invasive mechanical ventilation (hours), mean (SD)
169.5 (88.6)
Length of PICU stay (days), median (IQR)a
7.4 (5.7–9.0)
Glucose (mmol/L) during PICU admission, mean (SD)a
6.1 (0.8)
Episodes of glucose > 10 mmol/L, median (IQR)
0.0 (0.0–1.0)
Episodes of pCO2 > 6.4 kPa, median (IQR)a
12.0 (7.5–19.5)
Episodes of pCO2 < 4.7 kPa, median (IQR)
1.0 (0.0–2.0)
Episodes of pH > 7.45, median (IQR)
6.0 (4.0–11.5)
Episodes of pH < 7.35, median (IQR)
2.0 (0.0–4.0)
Episodes of SpO2 < 90%, median (IQR)
1.0 (0.0–2.0)
Episodes of SpO2 < 85%, median (IQR)a
0.0 (0.0–1.0)
Minimum FiO2 (%), median (IQR)
26.0 (25.0–30.0)
Maximum FiO2 (%), mean (SD)
88.6 (17.0)
Mean SpO2/FiO2 ratio, mean (SD)
2.5 (0.5)
Minimum SpO2/FiO2 ratio, mean (SD)a
1.1 (0.3)
Episodes of etCO2 < 3.5 kPa, median (IQR)
1.0 (0.0–4.0)
Episodes of etCO2 > 6.5 kPa, median (IQR)
5.0 (1.0–14.0)
Difference between PIP and PEEP (cmH2O), mean (SD)
15.9 (2.5)
Mean airway pressure (cmH2O), mean (SD)a
13.4 (1.8)
CPR cardiopulmonary resuscitation, etCO2 end-tidal carbon dioxide, ECMO extracorporeal membrane oxygenation, FiO2 fraction of inspired oxygen, PEEP positive end-expiratory pressure, PICU pediatric intensive care unit, PIP positive inspiratory pressure, PIM2 score Pediatric Index of Mortality 2 score, PIP positive inspiratory pressure, SpO2 oxygen saturation
aVariable eliminated in the Linear Regression and k-Nearest Neighbors models due to multicollinearity

Neurocognitive functioning

Neurocognitive outcomes are fully described elsewhere [35] and summarized in Online Resource 7. In brief, the patient group had significantly lower FSIQ (M = 95.3, SD = 15.9) than the control group (M = 105.1, SD = 15.1; p < .001, Cohen’s d = −0.59), and significantly poorer performance on the domains Speed and Attention (p = .03, d = −0.41) and Verbal Memory (p < .001, d = −0.60). To elucidate the potential relevance of patient and PICU-related characteristics for long-term adverse neurocognitive outcome after PICU admission and to explore the potential of machine learning, these three neurocognitive outcomes were selected as outcome measures.

Value of machine learning

Internal generalization

Table 2 displays the results regarding internal generalizability and performance of the models. For the majority of models, we found no significant difference in model performance on blind test data as compared to the training data, suggesting sufficient internal generalization of model performance. As exception, the Regression Trees model for Verbal Memory showed significantly higher performance in the blind test data as compared to the training data, suggesting insufficient internal generalization of model performance. The wide confidence intervals should be noted, with limited sensitivity for comparisons of internal model generalization.
Table 2
Cross-validated results and bootstrapped (R = 1000) test results
Outcome
Algorithm
R2 (%) training set
R2 (%) blind test set
95% CI of the R2 (%) blind test set
FSIQ
Linear Regression
24.1
25.9
0.0, 97.3
Regression Trees
19.7
65.3
19.2, 96.9
k-Nearest Neighbors
8.9
15.8
0.0, 58.4
Speed and attention
Linear Regression
43.9
53.5
1.6, 98.9
Regression Trees
31.4
70.2
12.2, 98.9
k-Nearest Neighbors
7.6
16.9
0.0, 63.8
Verbal memory
Linear Regression
41.0
50.6
4.0, 98.5
Regression Trees
10.8
76.7
23.0, 99.4
k-Nearest Neighbors
7.6
16.7
0.0, 66.5
FSIQ full-scale intelligence quotient

Performance

The reference Linear Regression models showed predictive value for FSIQ (R2 = 25.9%, 95%-CI 0.0–97.3%, p = .005), performance on the Speed and Attention domain (R2 = 53.5%, 95%-CI 1.6–98.9%, p < .001) and performance on the Verbal Memory domain (R2 = 50.6%, 95%-CI 4.0–98.5%, p < .001). As compared to the reference Linear Regression models, we found no significant differences in performance (on blind test data) for the Regression Trees and k-Nearest Neighbors machine learning models. Again, the wide confidence intervals should be noted, reflecting limited sensitivity for model performance comparisons.
Taken together, the Regression Trees model for Verbal Memory showed poor internal generalizability of model performance to new data, while both the Regression Trees and k-Nearest Neighbors models did not reveal added value in terms of model performance as compared to Linear Regression. These findings provide no evidence for added value of these machine learning models in the prediction of long-term neurocognitive outcome.

Prediction of long-term neurocognitive outcome

Considering that we did not find evidence for added value of the Regression Trees and k-Nearest Neighbor machine learning models, we used the Linear Regression reference models to provide insight in the variables that contribute to the prediction of long-term neurocognitive outcome (Table 3). The results show that lower FSIQ was predicted by lower birth weight and lower socioeconomic status (R2 = 25.9%, 95%-CI 0.0–97.3%). Poorer performance on the Speed and Attention domain was solely predicted by younger age at follow-up (R2 = 53.5%, 95%-CI 1.6–98.9%). Poorer performance on the Verbal Memory domain was predicted by lower birth weight, younger age at follow-up, and greater exposure to acidotic events (episodes of pH < 7.35; R2 = 50.6%, 95%-CI 4.0–98.5%).
Table 3
Results of the final multivariable linear regression models
Neurocognitive outcomes
Predictors
Standardized beta
Unstandardized beta (95% CI)
p-value
FSIQ
Total model
  
.005
Birth weight (grams)
0.24
0.004 (0.000, 0.008)
.047
Socioeconomic status
0.31
4.12 (0.97, 7.28)
.011
Speed and attention
Total model
  
< .001
Age at follow-up (years)
0.57
0.46 (0.29, 0.62)
< .001
Verbal memory
Total model
  
< .001
Birth weight (grams)
0.44
0.001 (0.000, 0.001)
< .001
Age at follow-up (years)
0.25
0.24 (0.04, 0.44)
.019
Episodes of pH < 7.35
−0.29
−0.06 (−0.10, −0.02)
.008
FSIQ full-scale intelligence quotient

Exploratory analysis

We further explored exposure to acidotic events (episodes of pH < 7.35). Acidosis (pH < 7.35) was observed in 47 of 65 patients (72.3%) and regarding acidosis severity, the following pH values were observed: pH 7.25–7.35, 196 observations in 47 patients; pH 7.20–7.25, 36 observations in 16 patients; pH < 7.20, 41 observations in 10 patients. In 247 (90.5%) observations, acidosis co-occurred with elevated pCO2, in one observation with elevated lactate, and in five observations with a combination of elevated pCO2 and elevated lactate. In 235 (86%) observations of acidosis, lactate was not measured. The pattern findings suggest a respiratory origin is more likely to explain the occurrence of acidosis as compared to a metabolic origin, although a combination cannot be ruled out due to the unavailability of lactate measurements for the majority of acidotic events.
The relation between verbal memory outcome and other aspects of acidosis exposure was also explored by multivariable linear regression analysis with backward elimination. The following independent pH variables were used: lowest pH value of each patient, mean pH value of each patient, and exposure to severe acidotic events (pH < 7.20). In addition, we also used exposure to hypercapnia (pCO2 > 6.4 kPa) as an independent variable. Results are displayed in Table 4. Lower mean pH values and greater exposure to elevated pCO2 levels were associated with poorer verbal memory outcome (p = .038 and p = .011, respectively).
Table 4
Exploratory analysis regarding acidotic events
Neurocognitive outcome
Predictors
Median (IQR)
Standardized beta
Unstandardized beta (95% CI)
p-value
Verbal memory
Lowest pH value for each patient
7.29 (7.21–7.36)
0.23
2.34 (−0.16, 4.85)
.07
Mean pH value for each patient
7.42 (7.41–7.44)
0.26
9.15 (0.52, 17.79)
.038
Episodes of pH < 7.20
0 (0–0)
−0.17
−0.06 (−0.15, 0.03)
.21
Episodes of pCO2 > 6.4 kPa
12.0 (7.5–19.5)
−0.32
−0.03 (−0.05, -0.01)
.011
pCO2 partial pressure of carbon dioxide

Discussion

This study aimed (1) to elucidate the potential relevance of patient and PICU-related characteristics for long-term adverse neurocognitive outcome after PICU admission for bronchiolitis, and (2) to perform a preliminary exploration of the potential of machine learning as compared to linear regression to improve neurocognitive outcome prediction in a relatively small sample of children after PICU admission. The results provide no evidence for the added value of machine learning models as compared to conventional linear regression analysis in the prediction of long-term neurocognitive outcome after PICU admission for bronchiolitis. As may be expected, linear regression analysis revealed that neurocognitive outcome was associated with demographic and perinatal characteristics (socioeconomic status, age at follow-up, and birth weight). Moreover, children with greater exposure to acidotic events during PICU admission for bronchiolitis had poorer verbal memory outcome. As the involvement of the central nervous system in the pathology of bronchiolitis is unlikely [26, 27], the relation between acidotic events and neurocognitive outcome may reflect either potentially harmful effects of acidosis itself, or reflect related processes such as hypercapnia, hypoxic, and/or ischemic events during PICU admission.
Given the large number of factors and mechanisms that have been proposed to contribute to long-term neurocognitive outcome of critically ill patients, characteristics of machine learning models (such as flexibility, ability to model non-linear relationships, more advanced inherent selection strategies) may provide potential to improve neurocognitive outcome prediction. We used machine learning in the current sample to perform a preliminary exploration of the potential value of machine learning to improve outcome prediction in a relatively smaller sample, although comparable in size to other post-PICU follow-up studies [53]. Regarding comparison of prediction models, we found no evidence for added value of the Regression Trees and k-Nearest Neighbors machine learning models as compared to conventional linear regression analysis. The wide confidence intervals, potentially reflecting the small sample size of the blind test set, provided limited sensitivity for model comparisons. Nevertheless, the findings suggest that machine learning models may not have added value in smaller sample sizes. Although there are examples of successful machine learning applications in small datasets [54], machine learning flourishes by large datasets not easily obtained in clinical settings [55]. This further stresses the importance of multicenter (international) collaborations [56] to pool clinical data and acquire larger datasets for clinical research into advanced outcome prediction using machine learning. In this study, model performance (assessed by R2) was not sufficient to have utility for individual outcome prediction. Nevertheless, it should be stressed that additional measures of model performance (e.g., precision and calibration) are critical to evaluate when evaluating the value of prediction models for individual outcome prediction [57]. In this study, we found no evidence for a typical pattern of overfitting (i.e., relatively high performance on training data combined with relatively low performance on blind test data). Conversely, a pattern of relatively high performance on the blind test data combined with relatively low performance on training data can be observed, indicating instable model performance. Considering the relatively larger number of predictors relative to the size of the study sample, more comprehensive data reduction and predictor selection methodology could decrease the amount of predictors for each model and potentially improve the performance of machine learning in future work, and is considered particularly important for smaller samples.
The results of our study further show that lower socioeconomic status was associated with lower intelligence after PICU admission for bronchiolitis. Abundant research has documented the relation between lower socioeconomic status and poorer neurocognitive functioning, of which the origin is matter of debate [18, 19, 28]. For example, poverty in early childhood and adverse environmental influences have been found related to neurocognitive functioning later in life [28]. In addition, literature shows that enriched environments throughout development influence brain plasticity and gene expression and resultant phenotypic cognitive traits [28]. We also observed that younger age at follow-up was associated with poorer neurocognitive functioning (i.e., poorer speed and attention and verbal memory). Most likely, this finding reflects a developmental effect, i.e., reflecting the commonly observed age-related improvements in neurocognitive functioning [42]. Children with younger age at follow-up also had shorter recovery time (r = .98), which could theoretically also have contributed to relatively poorer neurocognitive performance in younger children. Indeed, literature shows an association between younger age at follow-up and poorer neurocognitive functioning in some PICU subgroups, such as children admitted after heart- or heart-lung-transplantation [7], although contradicting findings have been reported in children and adolescents who survived meningococcal septic shock [20]. Furthermore, lower birth weight was associated with lower intelligence and poorer verbal memory. This result is consistent with existing work reporting an association between lower birth weight and poorer neurocognitive functioning [5860].
The findings of our study further suggest that greater exposure to acidotic events during PICU admission is associated with poorer verbal memory outcome. In experimental studies, several mechanisms have been proposed that may explain a potential negative effect of acidosis on the central nervous system, such as acidosis causing denaturation of proteins and nucleic acids, triggering cell swelling potentially leading to cellular edema and osmolysis, and inhibition of excitatory neurotransmission in the hippocampus, and influencing neuronal vulnerability indirectly by damaging glial cells [61, 62]. Although the translation of these findings from the literature to our study findings is unclear, our findings indicate that acidotic events may be implicated in negative effects on the central nervous system, whether or not through other neurotoxic processes such as hypercapnia, hypoxia, or ischemia. In our exploratory analyses, we found additional evidence indicating that higher pCO2 measurements, compatible with a respiratory origin of acidosis, were also related to poorer verbal memory outcome. Regardless of the exact mechanisms at play, our findings suggest that children with greater exposure to acidotic events are at risk of adverse long-term neurocognitive outcome after PICU admission for bronchiolitis, a finding that awaits replication in future prospective studies.
Bronchiolitis is a relatively mild indication for PICU admission that seldom manifests neurologically [26, 27] and is therefore not expected to affect neurocognitive functioning in itself. The observed adverse long-term neurocognitive outcomes may suggest that (a combination of) secondary consequences of bronchiolitis and/or PICU treatment may negatively affect outcomes after PICU admission. In previous work, we found no evidence for a relationship between exposure to sedatives, analgesics, anesthetics (per local protocol that was used at that time at our PICU) and a range of neurocognitive outcomes in the current sample [35]. In addition, duration of invasive mechanical ventilation was also not associated with neurocognitive outcomes [35]. In recent years, PICU therapy for bronchiolitis shifted to less invasive mechanical ventilation and more high-flow nasal cannula, with potential relevance for long-term outcome. Nevertheless, we found no association between invasive mechanical ventilation and neurocognitive outcomes, suggesting that the shift towards less invasive ventilation is unlikely to influence neurocognitive outcome. Furthermore, other factors such as hypoxic episodes, hypotension associated with mechanical ventilation, and metabolic derangements may have negatively affected children’s neurocognitive outcome after PICU admission [1214, 63]. As understanding of the exact nature and origin of difficulties in neurocognitive functioning is a prerequisite for successful prevention and intervention, the findings of our study highlight the importance of large prospective studies aimed at identifying the combination of factors that may account for adverse neurocognitive outcome in children admitted to the PICU for bronchiolitis, and for PICU admission in general.
Although prevention strategies, such as respiratory syncytial virus vaccine in pregnancy [64], show promising results, children will continue to be admitted to the PICU for bronchiolitis and for other admission indications. The findings of this study suggest that these children may be at risk of adverse neurocognitive outcome, even in the absence of a clear neurological manifestation of the underlying disease. Neurocognitive impairments are known to interfere with development in crucial outcome domains [811]. In addition, the results of our previous study [65], in which we investigated the same children included in the current study, showed that these children are at risk of long-term adverse daily life outcomes in terms of academic performance and health-related quality of life regarding school functioning 6–12 years after PICU admission for bronchiolitis. Furthermore, the findings of that study [65] suggest that lower intelligence may contribute to academic difficulties after PICU admission. Our findings underline the importance of long-term structured follow-up after PICU admission, even in the absence of underlying disease with neurological manifestation, enabling early identification and appropriate management of adverse outcomes. Furthermore, as it is unclear whether adverse neurocognitive outcomes can be catched up later in life, it may be warranted to continue follow-up monitoring into adulthood.
A limitation of our study is that a substantial number of eligible children (45.4%) did not participate in our study, mainly because they were not reached despite our efforts. However, we deem it unlikely that this has caused important selection bias because the study sample did not differ from the total cohort of eligible children in terms of demographic characteristics and illness severity. A second limitation relates to the operationalization of socioeconomic status as the average level of parental education. The use of parental education is only one attribute of the multifaceted construct of socioeconomic status, not accounting for the roles of, for example, income and level of professional functioning [66]. This may limit the generalizability of the study to communities with wide disparities according to for example race, ethnicity, economic opportunities, and/or insurance status. Furthermore, we acknowledge that the reported associations between risk factors and outcome may not reflect causal relationships [67]. Important to note, is that the number of acidotic events was determined on blood gas analyses measured based on clinical signs of respiratory distress. Therefore, the number of assessed blood gas analyses varied between patients based on the presentation of clinical state. In addition, we included both arterial and capillary blood gas and lactate measures, as only a minority of the children had an arterial line. Yet, capillary blood gases accurately reflect arterial pH and pCO2 in most PICU patients (in particular in hemodynamic stable patients) [68]. Another limitation of this study is that we did not perform external validation of the models, such as by an independently collected dataset sample of another hospital. Therefore, the hypothesis that acidotic events may increase the risk of adverse verbal memory outcome awaits replication in future work. At last, this study has modest sample size and hence had limited statistical power [69]. A strength of our study is that we extensively investigated patient and PICU-related characteristics in the relation between PICU admission and neurocognitive outcome. In addition, we focused on children admitted to the PICU for bronchiolitis, in an attempt to control for the confounding effect of underlying disease on outcome.

Conclusion

The findings of this study suggest that in children with previous PICU admission for bronchiolitis, (1) lower birth weight, younger age at follow-up, and lower socioeconomic are associated with poorer neurocognitive outcome; and (2) greater exposure to acidotic events during PICU admission is associated with poorer verbal memory outcome. Our study does not provide evidence for the added value of machine learning models as compared to conventional linear regression analysis in the prediction of long-term neurocognitive outcome in a relatively small sample of children with PICU admission. This study further highlights the importance of structured follow-up to monitor long-term outcome of children after PICU admission.

Acknowledgements

We thank all children and their parents who participated in this study.

Declarations

Ethics approval

This study was part of a larger study entitled “Long-term effects of sedative treatment on neurocognitive functioning in PICU survivors” that was approved by the medical ethical committee of the Amsterdam UMC, location AMC (W16_121#16.139 on April 15, 2016) and was conducted in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki [70].
Parents and children aged 12 years provided written informed consent for participation.

Competing interests

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Jetzt e.Med zum Sonderpreis bestellen!

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Jetzt bestellen und 100 € sparen!

e.Med Pädiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Pädiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen des Fachgebietes Pädiatrie, den Premium-Inhalten der pädiatrischen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Pädiatrie-Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Epstein D, Brill JE (2005) A history of pediatric critical care medicine. Pediatr Res 58(5):987–996PubMedCrossRef Epstein D, Brill JE (2005) A history of pediatric critical care medicine. Pediatr Res 58(5):987–996PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Namachivayam P, Shann F, Shekerdemian L, Taylor A, van Sloten I, Delzoppo C, Daffey C, Butt W (2010) Three decades of pediatric intensive care: who was admitted, what happened in intensive care, and what happened afterward. Pediatr Crit Care Med 11(5):549–555PubMedCrossRef Namachivayam P, Shann F, Shekerdemian L, Taylor A, van Sloten I, Delzoppo C, Daffey C, Butt W (2010) Three decades of pediatric intensive care: who was admitted, what happened in intensive care, and what happened afterward. Pediatr Crit Care Med 11(5):549–555PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Watson RS, Choong K, Colville G, Crow S, Dervan LA, Hopkins RO, Knoester H, Pollack MM, Rennick J, Curley MAQ (2018) Life after critical illness in children-toward an understanding of pediatric post-intensive care syndrome. J Pediatr 198:16–24PubMedCrossRef Watson RS, Choong K, Colville G, Crow S, Dervan LA, Hopkins RO, Knoester H, Pollack MM, Rennick J, Curley MAQ (2018) Life after critical illness in children-toward an understanding of pediatric post-intensive care syndrome. J Pediatr 198:16–24PubMedCrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Manning JC, Pinto NP, Rennick JE, Colville G, Curley MAQ (2018) Conceptualizing post intensive care syndrome in children-the PICS-p Framework. Pediatr Crit Care Med 19(4):298–300PubMedCrossRef Manning JC, Pinto NP, Rennick JE, Colville G, Curley MAQ (2018) Conceptualizing post intensive care syndrome in children-the PICS-p Framework. Pediatr Crit Care Med 19(4):298–300PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Pinto NP, Rhinesmith EW, Kim TY, Ladner PH, Pollack MM (2017) Long-term function after pediatric critical illness: results from the survivor outcomes study. Pediatr Crit Care Med 18(3):e122–e130PubMedCrossRef Pinto NP, Rhinesmith EW, Kim TY, Ladner PH, Pollack MM (2017) Long-term function after pediatric critical illness: results from the survivor outcomes study. Pediatr Crit Care Med 18(3):e122–e130PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat de Sonnaville ESV, Kӧnigs M, van Leijden O, Knoester H, van Woensel JBM, Oosterlaan J (2022) Intelligence outcome of pediatric intensive care unit survivors: a systematic meta-analysis and meta-regression. BMC Med 20(1):198PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef de Sonnaville ESV, Kӧnigs M, van Leijden O, Knoester H, van Woensel JBM, Oosterlaan J (2022) Intelligence outcome of pediatric intensive care unit survivors: a systematic meta-analysis and meta-regression. BMC Med 20(1):198PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, Martin LT, Kubzansky L, Harrington H, Poulton R, Caspi A (2009) Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: a test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry 166(1):50–57PubMedCrossRef Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, Martin LT, Kubzansky L, Harrington H, Poulton R, Caspi A (2009) Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: a test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry 166(1):50–57PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Gottfredson LS (1997) Why g matters: the complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 24(1):79–132CrossRef Gottfredson LS (1997) Why g matters: the complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 24(1):79–132CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Petrill SA, Wilkerson B (2000) Intelligence and achievement: a behavioral genetic perspective. Educ Psychol Rev 185–199 Petrill SA, Wilkerson B (2000) Intelligence and achievement: a behavioral genetic perspective. Educ Psychol Rev 185–199
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Strenze T (2006) Intelligence and socioeconomic success: a meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence 401–426 Strenze T (2006) Intelligence and socioeconomic success: a meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence 401–426
12.
13.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Hopkins RO, Jackson JC (2006) Long-term neurocognitive function after critical illness. Chest 130(3):869–878PubMedCrossRef Hopkins RO, Jackson JC (2006) Long-term neurocognitive function after critical illness. Chest 130(3):869–878PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Majnemer A, Limperopoulos C, Shevell M, Rohlicek C, Rosenblatt B, Tchervenkov C (2008) Developmental and functional outcomes at school entry in children with congenital heart defects. J Pediatr 153(1):55–60PubMedCrossRef Majnemer A, Limperopoulos C, Shevell M, Rohlicek C, Rosenblatt B, Tchervenkov C (2008) Developmental and functional outcomes at school entry in children with congenital heart defects. J Pediatr 153(1):55–60PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Vermunt LC, Buysse CM, Joosten KF, Duivenvoorden HJ, Hazelzet JA, Verhulst FC, Utens EM (2011) Survivors of septic shock caused by Neisseria meningitidis in childhood: psychosocial outcomes in young adulthood. Pediatr Crit Care Med 12(6):e302–e309PubMedCrossRef Vermunt LC, Buysse CM, Joosten KF, Duivenvoorden HJ, Hazelzet JA, Verhulst FC, Utens EM (2011) Survivors of septic shock caused by Neisseria meningitidis in childhood: psychosocial outcomes in young adulthood. Pediatr Crit Care Med 12(6):e302–e309PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Langenbacher D, Nield T, Poulsen MK (2001) Neurodevelopmental outcome of ECMO survivors at five years of age: the potential for academic and motor difficulties. J Spec Educ 156–160 Langenbacher D, Nield T, Poulsen MK (2001) Neurodevelopmental outcome of ECMO survivors at five years of age: the potential for academic and motor difficulties. J Spec Educ 156–160
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Ryerson LM, Guerra GG, Joffe AR, Robertson CM, Alton GY, Dinu IA, Granoski D, Rebeyka IM, Ross DB, Lequier L (2015) Survival and neurocognitive outcomes after cardiac extracorporeal life support in children less than 5 years of age: a ten-year cohort. Circ Heart Fail 8(2):312–321PubMedCrossRef Ryerson LM, Guerra GG, Joffe AR, Robertson CM, Alton GY, Dinu IA, Granoski D, Rebeyka IM, Ross DB, Lequier L (2015) Survival and neurocognitive outcomes after cardiac extracorporeal life support in children less than 5 years of age: a ten-year cohort. Circ Heart Fail 8(2):312–321PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Kachmar AG, Irving SY, Connolly CA, Curley MAQ (2018) A systematic review of risk factors associated with cognitive impairment after pediatric critical illness. Pediatr Crit Care Med 19(3):e164–e171PubMedCrossRef Kachmar AG, Irving SY, Connolly CA, Curley MAQ (2018) A systematic review of risk factors associated with cognitive impairment after pediatric critical illness. Pediatr Crit Care Med 19(3):e164–e171PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Vermunt LC, Buysse CM, Aarsen FK, Catsman-Berrevoets CE, Duivenvoorden HJ, Joosten KF, Hazelzet JA, Verhulst FC, Utens EM (2009) Long-term cognitive functioning in children and adolescents who survived septic shock caused by Neisseria meningitidis. Br J Clin Psychol 48(Pt 2):195–208PubMedCrossRef Vermunt LC, Buysse CM, Aarsen FK, Catsman-Berrevoets CE, Duivenvoorden HJ, Joosten KF, Hazelzet JA, Verhulst FC, Utens EM (2009) Long-term cognitive functioning in children and adolescents who survived septic shock caused by Neisseria meningitidis. Br J Clin Psychol 48(Pt 2):195–208PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Cleophas TJ, Zwinderman AH (2013) Machine learning in medicine. Springer, NetherlandsCrossRef Cleophas TJ, Zwinderman AH (2013) Machine learning in medicine. Springer, NetherlandsCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Miotto R, Li L, Kidd BA, Dudley JT (2016) Deep patient: an unsupervised representation to predict the future of patients from the electronic health records. Sci Rep 6:26094ADSPubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Miotto R, Li L, Kidd BA, Dudley JT (2016) Deep patient: an unsupervised representation to predict the future of patients from the electronic health records. Sci Rep 6:26094ADSPubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Lonsdale H, Jalali A, Ahumada L, Matava C (2020) Machine learning and artificial intelligence in pediatric research: current state, future prospects, and examples in perioperative and critical care. J Pediatr 221s:S3–s10 Lonsdale H, Jalali A, Ahumada L, Matava C (2020) Machine learning and artificial intelligence in pediatric research: current state, future prospects, and examples in perioperative and critical care. J Pediatr 221s:S3–s10
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Kamaleswaran R, Akbilgic O, Hallman MA, West AN, Davis RL, Shah SH (2018) Applying artificial intelligence to identify physiomarkers predicting severe sepsis in the PICU. Pediatr Crit Care Med 19(10):e495–e503PubMedCrossRef Kamaleswaran R, Akbilgic O, Hallman MA, West AN, Davis RL, Shah SH (2018) Applying artificial intelligence to identify physiomarkers predicting severe sepsis in the PICU. Pediatr Crit Care Med 19(10):e495–e503PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Johnson AE, Ghassemi MM, Nemati S, Niehaus KE, Clifton DA, Clifford GD (2016) Machine learning and decision support in critical care. Proc IEEE Inst Electr Electron Eng 104(2):444–466PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Johnson AE, Ghassemi MM, Nemati S, Niehaus KE, Clifton DA, Clifford GD (2016) Machine learning and decision support in critical care. Proc IEEE Inst Electr Electron Eng 104(2):444–466PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Pham H, Thompson J, Wurzel D, Duke T (2020) Ten years of severe respiratory syncytial virus infections in a tertiary paediatric intensive care unit. J Paediatr Child Health 56(1):61–67PubMedCrossRef Pham H, Thompson J, Wurzel D, Duke T (2020) Ten years of severe respiratory syncytial virus infections in a tertiary paediatric intensive care unit. J Paediatr Child Health 56(1):61–67PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Sweetman LL, Ng YT, Butler IJ, Bodensteiner JB (2005) Neurologic complications associated with respiratory syncytial virus. Pediatr Neurol 32(5):307–310PubMedCrossRef Sweetman LL, Ng YT, Butler IJ, Bodensteiner JB (2005) Neurologic complications associated with respiratory syncytial virus. Pediatr Neurol 32(5):307–310PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Bar S, Milanaik R, Adesman A (2016) Long-term neurodevelopmental benefits of breastfeeding. Curr Opin Pediatr 28(4):559–566PubMedCrossRef Bar S, Milanaik R, Adesman A (2016) Long-term neurodevelopmental benefits of breastfeeding. Curr Opin Pediatr 28(4):559–566PubMedCrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Polańska K, Jurewicz J, Hanke W (2015) Smoking and alcohol drinking during pregnancy as the risk factors for poor child neurodevelopment - a review of epidemiological studies. Int J Occup Med Environ Health 28(3):419–443PubMedCrossRef Polańska K, Jurewicz J, Hanke W (2015) Smoking and alcohol drinking during pregnancy as the risk factors for poor child neurodevelopment - a review of epidemiological studies. Int J Occup Med Environ Health 28(3):419–443PubMedCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Slater A, Shann F, Pearson G (2003) PIM2: a revised version of the Paediatric Index of Mortality. Intensive Care Med 29(2):278–285PubMedCrossRef Slater A, Shann F, Pearson G (2003) PIM2: a revised version of the Paediatric Index of Mortality. Intensive Care Med 29(2):278–285PubMedCrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Kneyber MC (2018) Ventilator-induced lung injury: does it occur in children? Minerva Anestesiol 84(5):626–631PubMedCrossRef Kneyber MC (2018) Ventilator-induced lung injury: does it occur in children? Minerva Anestesiol 84(5):626–631PubMedCrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat ALSG (2016) Advanced paediatric life support: a practical approach to emergencies, 6th edn. Wiley-Blackwell ALSG (2016) Advanced paediatric life support: a practical approach to emergencies, 6th edn. Wiley-Blackwell
35.
Zurück zum Zitat de Sonnaville ESV, Oosterlaan J, Ghiassi SA, van Leijden O, van Ewijk H, Knoester H, van Woensel JBM, Kӧnigs M (2023) Long-term neurocognitive outcomes after pediatric intensive care: exploring the role of drug exposure. Pediatr Res 94(2):603–610 de Sonnaville ESV, Oosterlaan J, Ghiassi SA, van Leijden O, van Ewijk H, Knoester H, van Woensel JBM, Kӧnigs M (2023) Long-term neurocognitive outcomes after pediatric intensive care: exploring the role of drug exposure. Pediatr Res 94(2):603–610
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Sattler JM (2008) Assessment of children: cognitive foundations, 5th edn. Sattler JM (2008) Assessment of children: cognitive foundations, 5th edn.
37.
Zurück zum Zitat van Ool JS, Hurks PPM, Snoeijen-Schouwenaars FM, Tan IY, Schelhaas HJ, Klinkenberg S, Aldenkamp AP, Hendriksen JGM (2018) Accuracy of WISC-III and WAIS-IV short forms in patients with neurological disorders. Dev Neurorehabil 21(2):101–107PubMedCrossRef van Ool JS, Hurks PPM, Snoeijen-Schouwenaars FM, Tan IY, Schelhaas HJ, Klinkenberg S, Aldenkamp AP, Hendriksen JGM (2018) Accuracy of WISC-III and WAIS-IV short forms in patients with neurological disorders. Dev Neurorehabil 21(2):101–107PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI (2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci 14(3):340–347PubMedCrossRef Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI (2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci 14(3):340–347PubMedCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Königs M, Weeda WD, van Heurn LW, Vermeulen RJ, Goslings JC, Luitse JS, Poll-The BT, Beelen A, van der Wees M, Kemps RJ, Catsman-Berrevoets CE, Oosterlaan J (2017) Pediatric traumatic brain injury affects multisensory integration. Neuropsychology 31(2):137–148PubMedCrossRef Königs M, Weeda WD, van Heurn LW, Vermeulen RJ, Goslings JC, Luitse JS, Poll-The BT, Beelen A, van der Wees M, Kemps RJ, Catsman-Berrevoets CE, Oosterlaan J (2017) Pediatric traumatic brain injury affects multisensory integration. Neuropsychology 31(2):137–148PubMedCrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Shallice T (1982) Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 298(1089):199–209ADSPubMedCrossRef Shallice T (1982) Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 298(1089):199–209ADSPubMedCrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Kingma A, van den Burg W (2005) Three parallel versions of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test for children Dutch version: instructions & normative data [Drie parallelversies van de 15-woordentest voor kinderen: handleiding & normering]. Stichting Kinderneuropsychologie Noord Nederland Kingma A, van den Burg W (2005) Three parallel versions of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test for children Dutch version: instructions & normative data [Drie parallelversies van de 15-woordentest voor kinderen: handleiding & normering]. Stichting Kinderneuropsychologie Noord Nederland
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Wechsler D (1991) Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III): Manual, 3rd edn. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX Wechsler D (1991)  Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III): Manual, 3rd edn. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Nutley SB, Söderqvist S, Bryde S, Humphreys K, Klingberg T (2010) Measuring working memory capacity with greater precision in the lower capacity ranges. Dev Neuropsychol 35(1):81–95PubMedCrossRef Nutley SB, Söderqvist S, Bryde S, Humphreys K, Klingberg T (2010) Measuring working memory capacity with greater precision in the lower capacity ranges. Dev Neuropsychol 35(1):81–95PubMedCrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat De Kieviet JF, Stoof CJ, Geldof CJ, Smits N, Piek JP, Lafeber HN, Van Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J (2013) The crucial role of the predictability of motor response in visuomotor deficits in very preterm children at school age. Dev Med Child Neurol 55(7):624–630PubMedCrossRef De Kieviet JF, Stoof CJ, Geldof CJ, Smits N, Piek JP, Lafeber HN, Van Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J (2013) The crucial role of the predictability of motor response in visuomotor deficits in very preterm children at school age. Dev Med Child Neurol 55(7):624–630PubMedCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Sterne JA, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG, Wood AM, Carpenter JR (2009) Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ 338:b2393PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sterne JA, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG, Wood AM, Carpenter JR (2009) Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ 338:b2393PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS (2007) Using multivariate statistics, 5th edn. Al-lyn & Beacon/Pearson Education, Boston, MA Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS (2007) Using multivariate statistics, 5th edn. Al-lyn & Beacon/Pearson Education, Boston, MA
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Field A (2017) Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics, 5th edn. SAGE Publications Ltd Field A (2017) Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics, 5th edn. SAGE Publications Ltd
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Fox J, Weisberg S (2019) An R companion to applied regression, 3rd edn. Sage, Thousand Oaks CA. Fox J, Weisberg S (2019) An R companion to applied regression, 3rd edn. Sage, Thousand Oaks CA.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuhn M (2008) Building predictive models in R using the caret package. J Stat Softw 28(5):1–26 Kuhn M (2008) Building predictive models in R using the caret package. J Stat Softw 28(5):1–26
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Kuhn M, Johnson K (2013) Applied predictive modeling. Springer Kuhn M, Johnson K (2013) Applied predictive modeling. Springer
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Maddux AB, Pinto N, Fink EL, Hartman ME, Nett S, Biagas K, Killien EY, Dervan LA, Christie LM, Luckett PM, Loftis L, Lackey M, Ringwood M, Smith M, Olson L, Sorenson S, Meert KL, Notterman DA, Pollack MM, Mourani PM, Watson RS (2020) Postdischarge outcome domains in pediatric critical care and the instruments used to evaluate them: a scoping review. Crit Care Med 48(12):e1313–e1321PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Maddux AB, Pinto N, Fink EL, Hartman ME, Nett S, Biagas K, Killien EY, Dervan LA, Christie LM, Luckett PM, Loftis L, Lackey M, Ringwood M, Smith M, Olson L, Sorenson S, Meert KL, Notterman DA, Pollack MM, Mourani PM, Watson RS (2020) Postdischarge outcome domains in pediatric critical care and the instruments used to evaluate them: a scoping review. Crit Care Med 48(12):e1313–e1321PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Shaikhina T, Lowe D, Daga S, Briggs D, Higgins R, Khovanova N (2015) Machine learning for predictive modelling based on small data in biomedical engineering. IFAC-Pap 48(20):469–474 Shaikhina T, Lowe D, Daga S, Briggs D, Higgins R, Khovanova N (2015) Machine learning for predictive modelling based on small data in biomedical engineering. IFAC-Pap 48(20):469–474
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Dhindsa K, Bhandari M, Sonnadara RR (2018) What’s holding up the big data revolution in healthcare? BMJ 363:k5357PubMedCrossRef Dhindsa K, Bhandari M, Sonnadara RR (2018) What’s holding up the big data revolution in healthcare? BMJ 363:k5357PubMedCrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Volovici V, Syn NL, Ercole A, Zhao JJ, Liu N (2022) Steps to avoid overuse and misuse of machine learning in clinical research. Nat Med 28(10):1996–1999PubMedCrossRef Volovici V, Syn NL, Ercole A, Zhao JJ, Liu N (2022) Steps to avoid overuse and misuse of machine learning in clinical research. Nat Med 28(10):1996–1999PubMedCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Steyerberg EW, Vickers AJ, Cook NR, Gerds T, Gonen M, Obuchowski N, Pencina MJ, Kattan MW (2010) Assessing the performance of prediction models: a framework for traditional and novel measures. Epidemiology 21(1):128–138PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Steyerberg EW, Vickers AJ, Cook NR, Gerds T, Gonen M, Obuchowski N, Pencina MJ, Kattan MW (2010) Assessing the performance of prediction models: a framework for traditional and novel measures. Epidemiology 21(1):128–138PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Breslau N, Chilcoat H, DelDotto J, Andreski P, Brown G (1996) Low birth weight and neurocognitive status at six years of age. Biol Psychiatry 40(5):389–397PubMedCrossRef Breslau N, Chilcoat H, DelDotto J, Andreski P, Brown G (1996) Low birth weight and neurocognitive status at six years of age. Biol Psychiatry 40(5):389–397PubMedCrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Starnberg J, Norman M, Westrup B, Domellöf M, Berglund SK (2018) Lower cognitive test scores at age 7 in children born with marginally low birth weight. Pediatr Res 83(6):1129–1135PubMedCrossRef Starnberg J, Norman M, Westrup B, Domellöf M, Berglund SK (2018) Lower cognitive test scores at age 7 in children born with marginally low birth weight. Pediatr Res 83(6):1129–1135PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Twilhaar ES, Wade RM, de Kieviet JF, van Goudoever JB, van Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J (2018) Cognitive outcomes of children born extremely or very preterm since the 1990s and associated risk factors: a meta-analysis and meta-regression. JAMA Pediatr 172(4):361–367PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Twilhaar ES, Wade RM, de Kieviet JF, van Goudoever JB, van Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J (2018) Cognitive outcomes of children born extremely or very preterm since the 1990s and associated risk factors: a meta-analysis and meta-regression. JAMA Pediatr 172(4):361–367PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Dulla CG, Dobelis P, Pearson T, Frenguelli BG, Staley KJ, Masino SA (2005) Adenosine and ATP link PCO2 to cortical excitability via pH. Neuron 48(6):1011–1023PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Dulla CG, Dobelis P, Pearson T, Frenguelli BG, Staley KJ, Masino SA (2005) Adenosine and ATP link PCO2 to cortical excitability via pH. Neuron 48(6):1011–1023PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Tombaugh GC, Sapolsky RM (1993) Evolving concepts about the role of acidosis in ischemic neuropathology. J Neurochem 61(3):793–803PubMedCrossRef Tombaugh GC, Sapolsky RM (1993) Evolving concepts about the role of acidosis in ischemic neuropathology. J Neurochem 61(3):793–803PubMedCrossRef
63.
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Kampmann B, Madhi SA, Munjal I, Simões EAF, Pahud BA, Llapur C, Baker J, Pérez Marc G, Radley D, Shittu E, Glanternik J, Snaggs H, Baber J, Zachariah P, Barnabas SL, Fausett M, Adam T, Perreras N, Van Houten MA, Kantele A, Huang LM, Bont LJ, Otsuki T, Vargas SL, Gullam J, Tapiero B, Stein RT, Polack FP, Zar HJ, Staerke NB, Duron Padilla M, Richmond PC, Koury K, Schneider K, Kalinina EV, Cooper D, Jansen KU, Anderson AS, Swanson KA, Gruber WC, Gurtman A (2023) Bivalent prefusion F vaccine in pregnancy to prevent RSV illness in infants. N Engl J Med 388(16):1451–1464PubMedCrossRef Kampmann B, Madhi SA, Munjal I, Simões EAF, Pahud BA, Llapur C, Baker J, Pérez Marc G, Radley D, Shittu E, Glanternik J, Snaggs H, Baber J, Zachariah P, Barnabas SL, Fausett M, Adam T, Perreras N, Van Houten MA, Kantele A, Huang LM, Bont LJ, Otsuki T, Vargas SL, Gullam J, Tapiero B, Stein RT, Polack FP, Zar HJ, Staerke NB, Duron Padilla M, Richmond PC, Koury K, Schneider K, Kalinina EV, Cooper D, Jansen KU, Anderson AS, Swanson KA, Gruber WC, Gurtman A (2023) Bivalent prefusion F vaccine in pregnancy to prevent RSV illness in infants. N Engl J Med 388(16):1451–1464PubMedCrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat de Sonnaville ESV, Kӧnigs M, Aarnoudse-Moens CSH, van Woensel JBM, Oosterlaan J, Knoester H (2023) Long-term follow-up of daily life functioning after pediatric intensive care unit admission. J Pediatr 260:113477PubMedCrossRef de Sonnaville ESV, Kӧnigs M, Aarnoudse-Moens CSH, van Woensel JBM, Oosterlaan J, Knoester H (2023) Long-term follow-up of daily life functioning after pediatric intensive care unit admission. J Pediatr 260:113477PubMedCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Shpitser I, Kudchadkar SR, Fackler J (2021) Causal inference from observational data: it is complicated. Pediatr Crit Care Med 22(12):1093–1096PubMedCrossRef Shpitser I, Kudchadkar SR, Fackler J (2021) Causal inference from observational data: it is complicated. Pediatr Crit Care Med 22(12):1093–1096PubMedCrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Harrison AM, Lynch JM, Dean JM, Witte MK (1997) Comparison of simultaneously obtained arterial and capillary blood gases in pediatric intensive care unit patients. Crit Care Med 25(11):1904–1908PubMedCrossRef Harrison AM, Lynch JM, Dean JM, Witte MK (1997) Comparison of simultaneously obtained arterial and capillary blood gases in pediatric intensive care unit patients. Crit Care Med 25(11):1904–1908PubMedCrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki (2013) Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310(20):2191–2194CrossRef World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki (2013) Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310(20):2191–2194CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Predicting long-term neurocognitive outcome after pediatric intensive care unit admission for bronchiolitis—preliminary exploration of the potential of machine learning
verfasst von
Eleonore S. V. de Sonnaville
Jacob Vermeule
Kjeld Oostra
Hennie Knoester
Job B. M. van Woensel
Somaya Ben Allouch
Jaap Oosterlaan
Marsh Kӧnigs
Publikationsdatum
06.11.2023
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Journal of Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2024
Print ISSN: 0340-6199
Elektronische ISSN: 1432-1076
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-023-05307-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2024

European Journal of Pediatrics 1/2024 Zur Ausgabe

Mit dem Seitenschneider gegen das Reißverschluss-Malheur

03.06.2024 Urologische Notfallmedizin Nachrichten

Wer ihn je erlebt hat, wird ihn nicht vergessen: den Schmerz, den die beim Öffnen oder Schließen des Reißverschlusses am Hosenschlitz eingeklemmte Haut am Penis oder Skrotum verursacht. Eine neue Methode für rasche Abhilfe hat ein US-Team getestet.

Ähnliche Überlebensraten nach Reanimation während des Transports bzw. vor Ort

29.05.2024 Reanimation im Kindesalter Nachrichten

Laut einer Studie aus den USA und Kanada scheint es bei der Reanimation von Kindern außerhalb einer Klinik keinen Unterschied für das Überleben zu machen, ob die Wiederbelebungsmaßnahmen während des Transports in die Klinik stattfinden oder vor Ort ausgeführt werden. Jedoch gibt es dabei einige Einschränkungen und eine wichtige Ausnahme.

Alter der Mutter beeinflusst Risiko für kongenitale Anomalie

28.05.2024 Kinder- und Jugendgynäkologie Nachrichten

Welchen Einfluss das Alter ihrer Mutter auf das Risiko hat, dass Kinder mit nicht chromosomal bedingter Malformation zur Welt kommen, hat eine ungarische Studie untersucht. Sie zeigt: Nicht nur fortgeschrittenes Alter ist riskant.

Begünstigt Bettruhe der Mutter doch das fetale Wachstum?

Ob ungeborene Kinder, die kleiner als die meisten Gleichaltrigen sind, schneller wachsen, wenn die Mutter sich mehr ausruht, wird diskutiert. Die Ergebnisse einer US-Studie sprechen dafür.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.