Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2020

Open Access 01.12.2020 | Research article

Measuring early childhood development with The Early Human Capability Index (eHCI): a reliability and validity study in China

verfasst von: Jin Zhao, Sally Anne Brinkman, Yunting Zhang, Yingquan Song, Chunling Lu, Mary Eming Young, Yue Zhang, Patrick Ip, Wenjie Shan, Fan Jiang

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2020

Abstract

Background

With the importance of early childhood development more recognized by the international society, low-cost and cross-culturally comparable measures of early childhood development is in great demand, both in China and worldwide. In this study, we aim to test the psychometrics of the Chinese version of The Early Human Capability Index (eHCI), which is designed as a measurement for school readiness in large population.

Methods

We evaluated the internal consistency, test-retest reliability, inter-rater reliability, factor structure, criterion-related validity, and discriminant validity of the eHCI in 20,324 preschool children in Shanghai. We also compared eHCI scores with test result of ASQ in 815 children in Yexian and EAP-ECDS in 6947 children in Daming.

Results

The ICC between parents and teachers were 0.83 and 0.63 for Literacy Numeracy and Overall Development. The confirmatory factor analyses showed good model fit (χ2 = 509,323, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.901; RMSEA = 0.038). The correlations between the scores of eHCI and other ECD metrics ranged between r = − 0.42 and r = 0.53. The scale discriminated between children’s developmental level based on sex, parental education, family income, family assets, and nutrition status.

Conclusions

Results from Chinese population suggested that eHCI is valid and reliable for measuring early childhood development in children aged 3–6 years. The eHCI can be applied to map the global distribution of early childhood development for allocating scarce resources to help those in greatest demand. Longitudinal studies are warranted to test its predictive validity for later outcomes.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
ECD
Early Childhood Development
EDI
Early Development Instrument
EHCI
Early Human Capacity Index
SCHEDULE-P
Shanghai Children’s Health, Education and Lifestyle Evaluation, Preschool
SDQ
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
ASQ: SE
Age & Stages Questionnaire: Social Emotional
ASQ
Age & Stages Questionnaire
EAP-ECDS
East Asia-Pacific Early Child Development Scales
ICC
Intraclass correlation coefficients

Background

The importance of early childhood development (ECD) remains profound. The capacities established during early childhood lay the foundation for physical, emotional, and intellectual wellbeing in middle childhood, throughout adolescence and into adulthood, even with multi-generational effects [1]. The 2007 and 2011 Lancet Series on Child Development in Developing Countries spearheaded the review of evidence linking early childhood development with adult health and wellbeing. The 2016 series considered new scientific evidence for intervention, and proposed pathways for implementation of early childhood development at scale [2]. Studies from across the globe, such as the Jamaica project, Perry Preschool and Abecedarian program, have demonstrated that interventions significantly improved childhood development and even later adult outcomes in the studied settings [35]. A meta-analysis, however, could not detect large effect sizes for the more recent and larger scale interventions [6], and the study suggests that ability of these measures for detecting effects could be one of the possible explanations. Tools for assessing early development used in small group trials, such as the Griffith and Bayley Scales of Infant Development, may not be effective in evaluating the impact of interventions implemented in large populations [7]. Traditionally, most measures of child development originate from the disciplines of pediatrics or developmental psychology, with focus on screening for developmental disability, which usually accounts for 10–15% of the whole population [8, 9]. However, there is evidence that more than 25% of children experience difficulties in learning, while they were not diagnosed as high-risk population by traditional clinical tools [10]. Moreover, many interventions implemented at scale are aimed at enhancing development, rather than identifying disabilities [11]. Therefore, a high-quality tool for measuring early childhood development is necessary to support the evaluation of early interventions. Such a tool would help to: evaluate children’s comprehensive traits, explore the protective factors that promote development and enhance child development at the population level [12].
Considering the limitations of clinical screening assessments, several tools have emerged to assess early childhood development at the population level. The Caregiver-Reported Early Development Instruments (CREDI) is developed for children under 3 years old and evaluates their early development [13]. As it was designed to function across a wide variety of culture, linguistic, and socioeconomic contexts, it has been promoted in 16 countries. The Early Childhood Development Index (ECDI) was launched by UNICEF as part of the Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys [14]. It contains 10 items covering the literacy–numeracy, learning, social–emotional, and physical development of children aged three and 4 years. The ECDI has been administrated in more than 60 low- and middle-income countries, and map the global early childhood development status.
Except for those tools developed for children in very early years, the concept of school readiness assessment is also considered to be an important indicator of early childhood development due to its effectiveness as a predictor of children’s future achievement [15]. If children are school ready, then they should be entering the education system with all the skills, capabilities, health and development to take advantage of the school learning environment and improve equity in achieving lifelong learning and full developmental potential among children [16]. The Early Development Instrument (EDI) is one of the few existing measurement that holistically evaluates the school readiness of children aged 3.5–6.5 years [17]. It was well-known as the main assessment tool in the Australian Early Development Census, which is implemented as a developmental census across the entire country once every 3 years [18]. However, the EDI is far from applied in international use, as it was originally designed for western culture. Cultural specificity is a key point in ECD concepts. Different aspects of culture (parenting practices, foods and social norms for example) can be both positive and negative for child development, however western developed instruments do not capture important aspects of child development in the Chinese culture and context. It is essential for any future population monitoring system of child development in China to be based on an instrument adapted to local culture and context. For example, the item of EDI “coming to school dressed appropriately” is intended to assessing children’s ability of organization, but most parents in poor countries and regions have no conditions to purchase “decent clothes” [16]. In view of these limitations, researchers are currently developing new scales that can better reflect child development across different cultures and contexts.
In 2013 the Early Human Capability Index (eHCI) was developed by Brinkman firstly in Tonga for impact evaluation of the school readiness component of the PERAL program [19]. The scale was designed to assess the comprehensive development of children aged 3–6 years at a population-level across diverse cultures. The original Tonga Early Human Capability Index contained 66 items in 9 domains including physical health, general verbal communication, cultural identity and spirituality, social and emotional well-being and skills, perseverance, approaches to learning, numeracy and concepts, formal literacy – reading and formal literacy – writing. It can be filled out by parents, teachers, social workers and other people familiar with the child. eHCI has been applied in other countries in the Pacific, South East Asia and Latin America. In 2014, the process of adapting the eHCI in China commenced. Through a process of discussions with experts in the fields of pediatric medicine and education, the instrument was adapted and revised item by item to conform to the cultural characteristics of China. Following this a series of pilots were conducted with particular attention paid to preventing any ceiling or floor effects for the Chinese population of children aged 3 through to 5 years of age. The aim of this paper is to validate the psychometrics of the Chinese version of the eHCI.

Methods

The development of EHCI in Chinese version

In 2014, under the guidance of Brinkman, an early childhood development specialist in Australia, the China Development Research Foundation (CDRF), in collaboration with the Shanghai Children’s Medical Center affiliated to Shanghai Jiaotong University School of Medicine, started working on the Chinese version of the early Human Capacity Index. Through discussions among experts in different fields such as pediatric medicine and education, the team translated each item into Chinese and made necessary amendments to reflect China’s cultural characteristics and avoid the ceiling and floor effects in the Chinese population. In 2015, based on the survey data of 3698 Chinese children, the developers carried out a Rasch model analysis of eHCI. The overall Chi-squared fit statistic of fit to the Rasch model was 2078.773 (df 540), p < 0.001 and the item fit residual was − 1.1258 (7.6952). The distribution of the item and person locations relative to one another on the same continuum is shown in Fig. 1 (Online). The Person Separation Index (the Rasch equivalent of Cronbach’s alpha indicating level of reliability) was 0.88509, which indicates high reliability. The power of the tests of fit was rated Excellent. The final scale has 62 entries, which can be completed by any person familiar with the child, such as parents, teachers, social workers, etc. The scale includes 9 dimensions: 1) verbal communication, 2) approaches to learning, 3) numeracy and concepts, 4) Reading, 5) writing, 6) cultural identity and spirituality, 7) social-emotional wellbeing, 8) perseverance, and 9) physical health. From these 9 domains an overall literacy and numeracy score is derived, as well as an overall development score, both ranging from 0 to 1 with 1 being the best score.

Study sample and data collection

This study used data to assess the reliability and validity of the eHCI mainly from the 2016 Shanghai Children’s Health, Education and Lifestyle Evaluation, Preschool (SCHEDULE-P) study. The 2016 SCHEDULE-P study was a cross-sectional survey that investigated the lifestyle, home environment and development of preschool children in 2016. The design, sampling and procedures of the survey have been described previously [20]. A representative sample of newly enrolled preschoolers in Shanghai kindergartens was obtained by stratified random sampling design. There were 20,899 children (age 36–58 months) from 191 kindergartens who enrolled in the study. From these, there were 20,324 families who consented to participate and the parents then completed the online questionnaire of the eHCI. The response rate was 97.2%.
To evaluate the discriminant validity of the eHCI, the demographic information of children and their family was also obtained in the survey. Age and sex of all participants were obtained from the Shanghai Kindergarten Registry Database of the Shanghai Education Committee and further confirmed by parents at the beginning of the study. Maternal education, paternal education, and annual household income were self-reported. The questionnaire included a family assets scale which sought information relating to the number of household cellphones, television, computers, cars and bathrooms [21]. Parents also reported the present height of the child, which was used to evaluate the nutrition status of children. The stunted children were defined as those with height-for-age less than 2 standard deviation using WHO standard for children aged 2–5 years. The principal-factor analysis was conducted to obtain a factor predicting the family assets. This same family asset scale was used in the Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) conducted by Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) to reflect family wealth.
To assess how constant eHCI scores remain from one occasion to another, a test-retest reliability survey was conducted in two Shanghai kindergartens not involved in the SCHEDULE-P study. Parents of 183 kindergarten children aged 3–6 years old completed the eHCI for a second time 9.1 [SD: 0.6] days after their first completion. In order to investigate the rater agreement, the eHCI ratings of 168 children from the two kindergartens were also compared between teachers and parent.
To test the correlations between the eHCI and other metrics of ECD by site, we used data from the SCHEDULE-P study, the One Sky program in Ye County and the kindergarten survey in Daming County. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) was used in the SCHEDULE-P study. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) and the Age & Stages Questionnaire: Social Emotional (ASQ: SE), two internationally recognized tools, were reported by parent to assess the psychosocial wellbeing status and the social-emotional development of the child in the SCHEDULE-P study [22]. The One Sky program conducted the study to describe the situation of the left-behind children in Ye County in August 2015. A total of 60 villages were selected from a list of all villages in Ye county provided by the Education Bureau of Ye county and the Bureau of Civil Affairs. All children aged 3 to 4-years and their families were interviewed in these villages. The Age & Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) and the eHCI was filled in by the caregiver of 1918 children. The ASQ was designed to measure child development in the domains of communication, gross and fine motor, problem-solving skills and personal-social skills [23]. The kindergarten survey in Daming County aimed to evaluate the early childhood development of children and the quality of preschool education in rural areas. Sixty-two kindergartens were randomly selected from all 217 kindergartens in Daming County in October 2017. The eHCI was reported by caregivers of all children (age 3–4 years) in the first year of kindergarten. The total sample of 6974 eHCI included 2203 paper and 4744 online questionnaires. The East Asia-Pacific Early Child Development Scales (EAP-ECDS) containing 99 items include seven domains: cognitive development; cultural knowledge and participation; language and emergent literacy, motor development; health, hygiene and safety; socio-emotional development; and approaches to learning [24]. The EAP-ECDS was tested in 1199 children on site by well-trained assessors. When interpreting some of the results presented in this paper it is important to note that for some aspects of the SDQ and the ASQ:SE higher scores represent children with greater development problems, which is opposite to how the other measures of child development are coded. The study was approved by the institutional review Board of the Shanghai Children’s medical center (SCMC), Shanghai Jiao Tong University (SCMCIRB-K2016022–01).

Statistical analysis

We conducted descriptive analysis on demographic characteristics using the SCHEDULE-P data. Reliability was assessed from analyses of internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and inter-rater agreement. Internal consistency of eHCI was assessed using Cronbach’s coefficient alpha. The intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) between eHCI scores by parents in two time points were calculated to assess the test-retest reliability. The ICC and paired t-test were calculated separately to assess the agreement and difference of eHCI scores rated by parents and teacher.
To validate the eHCI, we conducted the tests of structure validity, criterion-related validity and discriminant validity. Criterion-related validity of the eHCI was conducted by calculating its correlations with other metrics of ECD. Discriminant validity was tested through multi-level linear regression models assessing score differentials with respect to child sex, parental highest education, family income, quantiles of family assets, and nutrition status. The above analysis was conducted using Stata 14.2 (from StataCorp LP, College Station, Texas, U.S.A.). The Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) model was established to confirm the dimensionality of eHCI. The robust weighted least squares estimator (WLSMV) was used, as the item variables of the eHCI are categorical [25]. The CFA was operated in Mplus 8 (from Muthen & Muthen, Los Angeles, CA, U.S.A.).

Results

The mean age was 44.3 [SD: 3.6] months. Of those, 52.2% were boys and 47.8% were girls. Sample sizes and weighted demographic characteristics for Shanghai population were presented in Table 1.
Table 1
Descriptive characteristics of the sample (Total n = 20,324)
Categorical variable
N
%
Sex
 Boys
10,573
52.2
 Girls
9751
47.8
Parents’ highest education
 Secondary education and lower (Under Grade 12)
3089
19.7
 Tertiary education
13,313
63.3
 Post-graduate
3871
17.0
Annual household income
  < 100,000 RMB
4289
26.8
 100,000–300,000 RMB
9916
52.1
  > 300,000 RMB
4825
21.1
Nutrition status
 Non-stunting
19,851
97.4
 Stunting
428
2.6
Continuous variable
 
Mean ± SD
Age (month)
 
44.3 ± 3.6
Family assets
 
−0.1 ± 0.9
N = sample size; Mean / % = weighted mean or percentage adjusting for sampling design

Reliability

Internal consistency

The Cronbach’s α coefficient for Overall Development and Literacy Numeracy were respectively 0.87 and 0.84, and the others for the subscales were presented in Table 2.
Table 2
The Cronbach’s α coefficient for eHCI (n = 20,284)
 
items
Cronbach’s α coefficient
Literacy Numeracy
23
0.84
Overall Development
60
0.87
Subscales
 Verbal
6
0.61
 Physical
4
0.18
 Approaches
6
0.54
 Numeracy Concepts
12
0.75
 Reading
8
0.78
 Writing
3
0.53
 Cultural Spiritual
7
0.71
 Social Emotional
10
0.66
 Perseverance
4
0.54
EHCI Early Human Capability Index

Test-retest reliability

As reported in Table 3, the ICC between two scores of Literacy Numeracy and Overall Development in two time points were respectively 0.97 and 0.85, which were interpreted as excellent agreement in temporal stability.
Table 3
Intraclass Correlation Coefficients of test-retest and inter-rater for eHCI scores
 
Parent test-retest (n = 183)
Parent X Teacher (n = 168)
Literacy Numeracy
0.97
0.81
Overall Development
0.85
0.63
Subscales
 Verbal
0.71
0.18
 Physical
0.77
0.27
 Approaches
0.65
0.11
 Numeracy Concepts
0.92
0.78
 Reading
0.96
0.65
 Writing
0.92
0.71
 Cultural Spiritual
0.75
0.02
 Social Emotional
0.60
0.28
 Perseverance
0.48
0.35
EHCI Early Human Capability Index

Inter-rater reliability

The ICC between parents and teachers were 0.83 and 0.63 for Literacy Numeracy and Overall Development. As reported in Table 3, the lowest agreement between parents and teacher occurred in Approaches and Cultural Spiritual, the highest in the subscales of Numeracy Concepts. The results of the paired t-tests suggested that scores rated by teacher were significantly higher than that by parents for Literacy Numeracy (t = 3.51, df = 167, P = 0.001) and Overall Development (t = 2.29, df = 167, P = 0.023).

Validity

Factor structure

The confirmatory factor analysis was conducted, and the fit of model was good (χ2 = 509,323, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.901; RMSEA = 0.038). Factor loadings of items in each subscale are presented in Table 4. The majority of item’s factor loadings were above 0.7. Only the factor loading of item 50, 51, 55, 56 and 57 was below 0.4, which were all the reverse scored questions.
Table 4
Factor loadings of items in each subscale through confirmatory factor analysis (n = 20,271)
Items
Verbal
Approaches
Numeracy Concepts
Reading
1
0.622
   
2
0.851
   
3
0.930
   
4
0.842
   
5
0.946
   
6
0.834
   
7
 
0.411
  
8
 
0.725
  
9
 
0.653
  
10
 
0.474
  
11
 
0.746
  
12
 
0.807
  
13
  
0.751
 
14
  
0.785
 
15
  
0.764
 
16
  
0.701
 
17
  
0.742
 
18
  
0.669
 
19
  
0.658
 
20
  
0.744
 
21
  
0.708
 
22
  
0.605
 
23
  
0.735
 
24
  
0.765
 
Items
Writing
Cultural Spiritual
Social Emotional
Perseverance
Physical
25
   
0.966
 
26
   
0.986
 
27
   
0.847
 
28
   
0.663
 
29
   
0.701
 
30
   
0.597
 
31
   
0.921
 
32
   
0.939
 
33
0.726
    
34
0.910
    
35
0.797
    
36
 
0.665
   
37
 
0.710
   
38
 
0.496
   
39
 
0.795
   
40
 
0.821
   
41
 
0.815
   
42
 
0.800
   
43
  
0.640
  
44
  
0.612
  
45
  
0.768
  
46
  
0.711
  
47
  
0.778
  
48
  
0.731
  
49
  
0.731
  
50
  
0.049
  
51
  
0.160
  
52
  
0.576
  
53
   
0.748
 
54
   
0.890
 
55
   
0.308
 
56
   
0.348
 
57
    
0.020
58
    
0.636
60
    
0.650
Notes: Values are factor loadings in confirmatory factor analysis
As reported in Table 5, the correlations between the scores of eHCI and other ECD metrics ranged between r = − 0.42 and r = 0.53, and all were statistically significant. The direction of the correlation coefficient and the magnitude of the coefficient were all consistent with expectations, that is the direction of the coding (a high score represents poor development on some scales and low development on others) and the similarity of construct measured by the different instruments.
Table 5
Correlations between the scores of eHCI and other ECD metrics
 
Measure
Site
Year
N
Raw correlation
r
P value
Overall development
ASQ
Yexian
2016
1918
0.53
< 0.001
ASQ:SE
Shanghai
2016
16,478
−0.42
< 0.001
SDQ
Shanghai
2016
20,277
−0.45
< 0.001
EAP-ECDS
Daming
2018
1199
0.364
< 0.001
Numeracy and literacy
ASQ
Yexian
2016
1918
0.1254
< 0.001
ASQ:SE
Shanghai
2016
16,477
−0.24
< 0.001
SDQ
Shanghai
2016
20,276
−0.26
< 0.001
EAP-ECDS
Daming
2018
1199
0.445
< 0.001
Notes: EHCI Early Human Capability Index, ECD Early Child Development, ASQ Age & Stages Questionnaire, ASQ:SE Age & Stages Questionnaire: Social Emotional, SDQ Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, EAP-ECDS East Asia-Pacific Early Child Development Scales
The SDQ and the ASQ:SE higher scores represent children with greater development problems, which is opposite to how the other measures are coded

Discriminant validity

Differences in eHCI scores across several sociodemographic subgroups were shown in Table 6. Girls scored 0.025 (SE: 0.002) higher than boys in Overall Development and 0.017 (SE: 0.002) higher in Literacy Numeracy, adjusted for age, SES factors and nutrition status. Significantly higher scores were achieved by children with higher parental education, and in wealthier families. Compared with those children in normal nutrition status, stunted children scored − 0.04 (SE: 0.006) lower in Overall Development and − 0.03 (SE: 0.009) lower in Literacy Numeracy sifnificantly.
Table 6
Associations between demographic and social economic status and eHCI scores
 
Overall Development
Literacy Numeracy
 
N
Age-adjusted
Multivariate-adjusteda
N
Age-adjusted
Multivariate-adjusteda
Sex
20,232
  
20,231
  
 boys
 
Ref.
Ref.
 
Ref.
Ref.
 girls
 
0.026***
0.025***
 
0.019***
0.017***
  
(0.002)
(0.002)
 
(0.002)
(0.002)
Parental education
20,182
  
20,181
  
 Secondary education and lower (Under Grade 12)
 
Ref.
Ref.
 
Ref.
Ref.
 Tertiary education
 
0.052***
0.036***
 
0.116***
0.092***
  
(0.002)
(0.003)
 
(0.003)
(0.004)
 Post-graduate
 
0.086***
0.059***
 
0.179***
0.138***
  
(0.003)
(0.003)
 
(0.004)
(0.005)
Annual household income
18,950
  
18,949
  
  < 100,000 RMB
 
Ref.
Ref.
 
Ref.
Ref.
 100,000–300,000 RMB
 
0.034***
0.017***
 
0.071***
0.034***
  
(0.002)
(0.002)
 
(0.003)
(0.003)
  > 300,000
 
0.067***
0.040***
 
0.123***
0.068***
  
(0.002)
(0.003)
 
(0.003)
(0.004)
Family assets quantile
19,030
  
19,029
  
 1[poorest]
 
Ref.
Ref.
 
Ref.
Ref.
 2
 
0.016***
0.003
 
0.032***
0.004
  
(0.002)
(0.002)
 
(0.003)
(0.003)
 3
 
0.027***
0.008***
 
0.049***
0.013***
  
(0.002)
(0.002)
 
(0.003)
(0.003)
 4[richest]
 
0.036***
0.012***
 
0.054***
0.010**
  
(0.002)
(0.002)
 
(0.003)
(0.004)
Nutrition status
20,227
     
 Non-stunting
 
Ref.
Ref.
 
Ref.
Ref.
Stunting
 
−0.06***
−0.04***
 
−0.08***
−0.03**
  
(0.006)
(0.006)
 
(0.009)
(0.009)
Values are linear regression coefficients (95% CI)
aAdjusted for age and all characteristics included in table
*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001

Discussion

The psychometric properties of eHCI were evaluated in a representative sample of children aged 3–4 years from all districts of Shanghai. Results of the present study suggest the eHCI is psychometrically sound for Chinese children.
In terms of reliability indicators, the α coefficient indicates good internal consistency sufficient for group comparison other than the domain of physical [26]. The physical subscale was designed to understand children’s disability, health status and behavior. The four items in the subscale are: “Is this child frequently sickly? “, “Does this child have good hygiene i.e. always wash their hands after toileting?”, “Does this child have any disabilities/special needs?”,“Does this child have a regular diet?”, are not strongly correlated with each other. Perhaps indicating that these physical factors act mainly as independent characteristics rather than as a scale of physical development. An ICC above 0.75 is considered as excellent [27]. The result of our reliability analysis suggested that eHCI had good internal consistency and temporal stability. However, the inter-rater agreement in the present analysis was more variable, with subscales related to Literacy Numeracy showing excellent consistency between scores rated by parents and teachers, and the others showing greater heterogeneity in responses. Since the items in Numeracy Concepts, Reading, and Writing are relatively objective indicators, it is reasonable that scores in those aspects were more consistent between parents and teachers. These results are consistent with the reported reliability of other measures of child development and the reasons for inconsistent are likely to be related to parent, teacher and child factors as well as context (for example; parental knowledge of child development, parent literacy levels, parental engagement in the school system, teacher qualifications and knowledge of development, teachers experience across different socioeconomic settings, child behavior being different in the school compared to home due to shyness or other factors). The paired t-test results suggested that teacher scored higher than parents for the same children. For example, the items in cultural spiritual are “Does this child talk politely?”, and “Is this child good to his or her parents?”. It may be because children act differently in kindergarten than at home. It also may be because parents expected too much of their children. We cannot draw a conclusion without deeper exploration of the reason behind the disagreement. In the future, when using the eHCI or other measures of child development it will be important to distinguish the raters prior to scores being compared across different populations.
The results of confirmatory factor analysis supported the underlying structure of the eHCI. The model fit demonstrated that the extracted factors from all items are capable of assessing the different developmental domains in Chinese children. All but five items have high factor loadings. Those five are reverse coded question: kick, bite or hit adults or other children; impatient; need constant reminding to finish something off; get easily distracted from a task; frequently sickly. Even though the factor loadings of the reverse coded items were lower than expected, it may be important to keep the items worded in a negative fashion. There is evidence to suggest that respondents get into a pattern of response and reversing the direction of a question requires deeper thinking, however others in survey methodology would recommend keeping all survey items in the same direction for simplicity and to reduce confusion [17, 28]. This may be something worth exploring further with future use of the eHCI.
The eHCI showed significant correlations with other metrics covering different domains of child development, such as Age & Stages Questionnaire (gross motor, fine motor, communication, problem-solving, social-personal), Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (psychosocial well-being), etc. However, those metrics are inclined to screen the individual with high-risk of development. The eHCI was designed to monitor the comprehensive abilities of children at population level. As such, we would not have expected correlations larger than what was found.
The discriminant validity of eHCI with demographic characteristics was also presented in the results. The eHCI scores of girls were significantly more than those of boys, consistent with the conclusion of other studies that girls mature earlier than boys [29]. The results also suggest that higher eHCI scores appeared in the groups with higher socioeconomic status, in keeping with prior research [30]. A large body of researches has found stunting are negatively related to early childhood development [31, 32], which is also certified using eHCI scale in this study. The significant association between eHCI scores and demographic characteristics verified that eHCI could detect the development heterogeneity of different populations.
This study has several limitations that deserve mention. First, although the eHCI was proved to be a feasible and comprehensive tool for identifying the developmental level of Chinese children, the overall sample was not representative of the national population, even though children from migrant workers from rural areas in Shanghai were included within this sample. Second, although the eHCI could be applied as an instrument for monitoring and to compare the status of early childhood development in different populations worldwide for its cross-culture design, it is not meant to replace traditional screening or diagnostic tools for delayed development. The eHCI emphasizes improving early childhood development at a population level, rather than diagnosing individual children as abnormal. Future studies should take this into consideration according to their target population and goal. Third, although the reliability and validity of eHCI has been tested in this study, there is still no evidence to verify eHCI as a reliable predictor of long-term indicators of academic or working achievement, such as education level, income, and crimes. Longitudinal studies are warranted to test its predictive validity for later outcomes.

Conclusions

The results of reliability and validity analysis suggest that the eHCI is a valid measurement to assess the overall development of Chinese children aged 3–6 years. It has enabled us to monitor the developmental trajectories of children, implement evidence-based interventions to improve their school readiness, and will ultimately support the evaluation of those interventions. The valuable aspect is that the eHCI can be applied to children from diverse cultural backgrounds, which makes it possible to map the global distribution of early childhood development for allocating scarce resources to help those in greatest demand. In the future, longitudinal studies will be conducted to identify its ability to predict important outcomes in later life.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.
Parents provided online informed consent for their own participation as well as the participation of their children before they began to fill in the questionnaire. The information involving the design of survey, voluntary nature of participation, what participation entails, risks and data security was presented. The invited participants can choose to click on the button indicating that they had read the information and agreed to participate or disagreed to participate. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Shanghai Children’s Medical Center (SCMC), Shanghai Jiao Tong University (SCMCIRB-K2016022–01).
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Black MM, Walker SP, Fernald LCH, Andersen CT, DiGirolamo AM, Lu C, et al. Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):77–90.PubMedCrossRef Black MM, Walker SP, Fernald LCH, Andersen CT, DiGirolamo AM, Lu C, et al. Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):77–90.PubMedCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Richter LM, Daelmans B, Lombardi J, Heymann J, Boo FL, Behrman JR, et al. Investing in the foundation of sustainable development: pathways to scale up for early childhood development. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):103–18.PubMedCrossRef Richter LM, Daelmans B, Lombardi J, Heymann J, Boo FL, Behrman JR, et al. Investing in the foundation of sustainable development: pathways to scale up for early childhood development. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):103–18.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Paul G, James H, Rodrigo P, Arianna Z, Christel V, Susan W, et al. Labor market returns to an early childhood stimulation intervention in Jamaica. Science. 2014;344(6187):998.CrossRef Paul G, James H, Rodrigo P, Arianna Z, Christel V, Susan W, et al. Labor market returns to an early childhood stimulation intervention in Jamaica. Science. 2014;344(6187):998.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Schweinhart LJ. Significant benefits: the High/ scope Perry preschool study through age 27. Monographs of the High/scope Educational Research Foundation Number; 1993. Schweinhart LJ. Significant benefits: the High/ scope Perry preschool study through age 27. Monographs of the High/scope Educational Research Foundation Number; 1993.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell FA, Helms R, Sparling JJ, Ramey CT. Early-childhood programs and success in school: the abecedarian study. State Univ N Y. 1998:145–66. Campbell FA, Helms R, Sparling JJ, Ramey CT. Early-childhood programs and success in school: the abecedarian study. State Univ N Y. 1998:145–66.
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Mccoy DC, Sudfeld CR, Bellinger DC, Muhihi A, Ashery G, Weary TE, et al. Development and validation of an early childhood development scale for use in low-resourced settings. Popul Health Metrics. 2017;15(1):3.CrossRef Mccoy DC, Sudfeld CR, Bellinger DC, Muhihi A, Ashery G, Weary TE, et al. Development and validation of an early childhood development scale for use in low-resourced settings. Popul Health Metrics. 2017;15(1):3.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Aylward GP. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. Hoboken: Wiley; 2010. p. 357–8. Aylward GP. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. Hoboken: Wiley; 2010. p. 357–8.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Lobello SG. A short form of the Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence-revised. J Sch Psychol. 1991;29(3):229–36.CrossRef Lobello SG. A short form of the Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence-revised. J Sch Psychol. 1991;29(3):229–36.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Rimm-Kaufman SE, Pianta RC, Cox MJ. Teachers’ judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Child Res Q. 2000;15(2):147–66.CrossRef Rimm-Kaufman SE, Pianta RC, Cox MJ. Teachers’ judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Child Res Q. 2000;15(2):147–66.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Engle PL, Fernald LCH, Alderman H, Behrman J, O'Gara C, Yousafzai A, Iltus S. Strategies for reducing inequalities and improving developmental outcomes for young children in low-income and middle-income countries. The Lancet. 2011;378(9799):1339–53. Engle PL, Fernald LCH, Alderman H, Behrman J, O'Gara C, Yousafzai A, Iltus S. Strategies for reducing inequalities and improving developmental outcomes for young children in low-income and middle-income countries. The Lancet. 2011;378(9799):1339–53.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Ntuli E, Nyarambi A, Traore M. Assessment in early childhood education: threats and challenges to effective assessment of immigrant children. J Res Spec Educ Needs. 2015;14(4):221–8.CrossRef Ntuli E, Nyarambi A, Traore M. Assessment in early childhood education: threats and challenges to effective assessment of immigrant children. J Res Spec Educ Needs. 2015;14(4):221–8.CrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat McCoy DC, Waldman M, Fink G. Measuring early childhood development at a global scale: evidence from the caregiver-reported early development instruments. Early Child Res Q. 2018;45:58–68.CrossRef McCoy DC, Waldman M, Fink G. Measuring early childhood development at a global scale: evidence from the caregiver-reported early development instruments. Early Child Res Q. 2018;45:58–68.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat UNICEF. The formative years: UNICEF’s work on measuring early childhooddevelopment.; 2014. UNICEF. The formative years: UNICEF’s work on measuring early childhooddevelopment.; 2014.
15.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Brinkman S, Thanh VB. Early childhood development in Tonga : baseline results from the Tongan early human capability index; World Bank studies;. World Bank. Washington, DC©: World Bank; 2017.CrossRef Brinkman S, Thanh VB. Early childhood development in Tonga : baseline results from the Tongan early human capability index; World Bank studies;. World Bank. Washington, DC©: World Bank; 2017.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Janus M, Offord DR. Development and psychometric properties of the early development instrument (EDI): a measure of Children's school readiness. Can J Behav Sci. 2007;39(1):1–22.CrossRef Janus M, Offord DR. Development and psychometric properties of the early development instrument (EDI): a measure of Children's school readiness. Can J Behav Sci. 2007;39(1):1–22.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Brinkman SA, Gregory TA, Sharon G, Lynch JW, Matthew H. Data resource profile: the Australian early development index (AEDI). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(4):1089–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Brinkman SA, Gregory TA, Sharon G, Lynch JW, Matthew H. Data resource profile: the Australian early development index (AEDI). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(4):1089–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Brinkman SA, Vu BT. Early childhood development in Tonga : baseline results from the Tongan early human capability index. World Bank Group: Washington, D.C; 2016.CrossRef Brinkman SA, Vu BT. Early childhood development in Tonga : baseline results from the Tongan early human capability index. World Bank Group: Washington, D.C; 2016.CrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhao J, Zhang Y, Jiang F, Ip P, Ho FKW, Zhang Y, et al. Excessive Screen Time and Psychosocial Well-Being: The Mediating Role of Body Mass Index, Sleep Duration, and Parent-Child Interaction. J Pediatr. 2018;202:157–62 e1.PubMedCrossRef Zhao J, Zhang Y, Jiang F, Ip P, Ho FKW, Zhang Y, et al. Excessive Screen Time and Psychosocial Well-Being: The Mediating Role of Body Mass Index, Sleep Duration, and Parent-Child Interaction. J Pediatr. 2018;202:157–62 e1.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat OECD. PISA 2009 Assessment framework – key competencies in reading, MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE 2009. OECD. PISA 2009 Assessment framework – key competencies in reading, MATHEMATICS AND SCIENCE 2009.
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman A, Goodman R. Strengths and difficulties questionnaire as a dimensional measure of child mental health. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(4):400–3.PubMedCrossRef Goodman A, Goodman R. Strengths and difficulties questionnaire as a dimensional measure of child mental health. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(4):400–3.PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Squires J, Bricker D, Potter L. Revision of a parent-completed developmental screening tool: ages and stages questionnaires. J Pediatr Psychol. 1997;22(3):313–28.PubMedCrossRef Squires J, Bricker D, Potter L. Revision of a parent-completed developmental screening tool: ages and stages questionnaires. J Pediatr Psychol. 1997;22(3):313–28.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Jing Z, Li Z, Rao NJGE. The early developmental status and differences of young children in China: a report from the EAP—ECDS validation study; 2018. Jing Z, Li Z, Rao NJGE. The early developmental status and differences of young children in China: a report from the EAP—ECDS validation study; 2018.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Liang X, Yang Y. An evaluation of WLSMV and Bayesian methods for confirmatory factor analysis with categorical indicators. Int J Quant Res Educ. 2014;2(1):17–38. Liang X, Yang Y. An evaluation of WLSMV and Bayesian methods for confirmatory factor analysis with categorical indicators. Int J Quant Res Educ. 2014;2(1):17–38.
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Murphy KRDC. Psychological testing: principles and applications. 4th ed. New Jersey: Prentice Hall International Inc.; 1996. Murphy KRDC. Psychological testing: principles and applications. 4th ed. New Jersey: Prentice Hall International Inc.; 1996.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Cicchetti DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(4):284–90.CrossRef Cicchetti DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(4):284–90.CrossRef
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Bert W, Hans B, Niels S. Reversed item bias: an integrative model. Psychol Methods. 2013;18(3):320–34.CrossRef Bert W, Hans B, Niels S. Reversed item bias: an integrative model. Psychol Methods. 2013;18(3):320–34.CrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Galsworthy MJ, Dionne G, Dale PS, Plomin R. Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Dev Sci. 2010;3(2):206–15.CrossRef Galsworthy MJ, Dionne G, Dale PS, Plomin R. Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Dev Sci. 2010;3(2):206–15.CrossRef
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53(1):371–99.PubMedCrossRef Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53(1):371–99.PubMedCrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat McCoy DC, Peet ED, Ezzati M, Danaei G, Black MM, Sudfeld CR, et al. Early childhood developmental status in low- and middle-income countries: national, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modeling. PLoS Med. 2016;13(6):e1002034.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef McCoy DC, Peet ED, Ezzati M, Danaei G, Black MM, Sudfeld CR, et al. Early childhood developmental status in low- and middle-income countries: national, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modeling. PLoS Med. 2016;13(6):e1002034.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Britto PR, Lye SJ, Proulx K, Yousafzai AK, Matthews SG, Vaivada T, et al. Nurturing care: promoting early childhood development. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):91–102.PubMedCrossRef Britto PR, Lye SJ, Proulx K, Yousafzai AK, Matthews SG, Vaivada T, et al. Nurturing care: promoting early childhood development. Lancet. 2017;389(10064):91–102.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Measuring early childhood development with The Early Human Capability Index (eHCI): a reliability and validity study in China
verfasst von
Jin Zhao
Sally Anne Brinkman
Yunting Zhang
Yingquan Song
Chunling Lu
Mary Eming Young
Yue Zhang
Patrick Ip
Wenjie Shan
Fan Jiang
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2020
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2020
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-020-02210-w

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2020

BMC Pediatrics 1/2020 Zur Ausgabe

Mit dem Seitenschneider gegen das Reißverschluss-Malheur

03.06.2024 Urologische Notfallmedizin Nachrichten

Wer ihn je erlebt hat, wird ihn nicht vergessen: den Schmerz, den die beim Öffnen oder Schließen des Reißverschlusses am Hosenschlitz eingeklemmte Haut am Penis oder Skrotum verursacht. Eine neue Methode für rasche Abhilfe hat ein US-Team getestet.

Reanimation bei Kindern – besser vor Ort oder während Transport?

29.05.2024 Reanimation im Kindesalter Nachrichten

Zwar scheint es laut einer Studie aus den USA und Kanada bei der Reanimation von Kindern außerhalb einer Klinik keinen Unterschied für das Überleben zu machen, ob die Wiederbelebungsmaßnahmen während des Transports in die Klinik stattfinden oder vor Ort ausgeführt werden. Jedoch gibt es dabei einige Einschränkungen und eine wichtige Ausnahme.

Alter der Mutter beeinflusst Risiko für kongenitale Anomalie

28.05.2024 Kinder- und Jugendgynäkologie Nachrichten

Welchen Einfluss das Alter ihrer Mutter auf das Risiko hat, dass Kinder mit nicht chromosomal bedingter Malformation zur Welt kommen, hat eine ungarische Studie untersucht. Sie zeigt: Nicht nur fortgeschrittenes Alter ist riskant.

Begünstigt Bettruhe der Mutter doch das fetale Wachstum?

Ob ungeborene Kinder, die kleiner als die meisten Gleichaltrigen sind, schneller wachsen, wenn die Mutter sich mehr ausruht, wird diskutiert. Die Ergebnisse einer US-Studie sprechen dafür.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.