Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Molecular Autism 1/2021

Open Access 01.12.2021 | Research

Genetic and morphological estimates of androgen exposure predict social deficits in multiple neurodevelopmental disorder cohorts

verfasst von: Brooke G. McKenna, Yongchao Huang, Kévin Vervier, Dabney Hofammann, Mary Cafferata, Seima Al-Momani, Florencia Lowenthal, Angela Zhang, Jin-Young Koh, Savantha Thenuwara, Leo Brueggeman, Ethan Bahl, Tanner Koomar, Natalie Pottschmidt, Taylor Kalmus, Lucas Casten, Taylor R. Thomas, Jacob J. Michaelson

Erschienen in: Molecular Autism | Ausgabe 1/2021

Abstract

Background

Neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) display a strong male bias. Androgen exposure is profoundly increased in typical male development, but it also varies within the sexes, and previous work has sought to connect morphological proxies of androgen exposure, including digit ratio and facial morphology, to neurodevelopmental outcomes. The results of these studies have been mixed, and the relationships between androgen exposure and behavior remain unclear.

Methods

Here, we measured both digit ratio masculinity (DRM) and facial landmark masculinity (FLM) in the same neurodevelopmental cohort (N = 763) and compared these proxies of androgen exposure to clinical and parent-reported features as well as polygenic risk scores.

Results

We found that FLM was significantly associated with NDD diagnosis (ASD, ADHD, ID; all \(p<0.05\)), while DRM was not. When testing for association with parent-reported problems, we found that both FLM and DRM were positively associated with concerns about social behavior (\(\rho =0.19\), \(p=0.004\); \(\rho =0.2\), \(p=0.004\), respectively). Furthermore, we found evidence via polygenic risk scores (PRS) that DRM indexes masculinity via testosterone levels (\(t=4.0\), \(p=8.8\times 10^{-5}\)), while FLM indexes masculinity through a negative relationship with sex hormone binding globulin (SHBG) levels (\(t=-3.3\), \(p=0.001\)). Finally, using the SPARK cohort (N = 9419) we replicated the observed relationship between polygenic estimates of testosterone, SHBG, and social functioning (\(t=-2.3\), \(p=0.02\), and \(t=4.2\), \(p={3.2\times 10^{-5}}\) for testosterone and SHBG, respectively). Remarkably, when considered over the extremes of each variable, these quantitative sex effects on social functioning were comparable to the effect of binary sex itself (binary male: \(-0.22\pm 0.05\); testosterone: \(-0.35\pm 0.15\) from 0.1%-ile to 99.9%-ile; SHBG: \(0.64\pm 0.15\) from 0.1%-ile to 99.9%-ile).

Limitations

In the devGenes and SPARK cohorts, our analyses rely on indirect, rather than direct measurement of androgens and related molecules.

Conclusions

These findings and their replication in the large SPARK cohort lend support to the hypothesis that increasing net androgen exposure diminishes capacity for social functioning in both males and females.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1186/​s13229-021-00450-w.
Brooke G. McKenna and Yongchao Huang have contributed equally to this work

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
2D:4D ratio
second and fourth digit ratio
ADHD
attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder
ASD
autism spectrum disorder
CI
confidence interval
DNA
deoxyribonucleic acid
DRM
digit ratio masculinity
FLM
facial landmark masculinity
GWAS
genome-wide association study
HWE
Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium
ID
intellectual disability
LD
linkage disequilibrium
MAF
minor allele frequency
MDS
multidimensional scaling
NDD
neurodevelopmental disorder
PRS
polygenic risk score
QC
quality control
SCQ
social communication questionnaire
SHBG
sex hormone binding globulin
SNP
single nucleotide polymorphism

Introduction

Most neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) present more often in males than females [1], and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in particular shows a striking sex bias with a 4:1 male-to-female ratio. However, despite the evidence that male sex—and masculinity more generally, as posited by the “extreme male brain” theory [2]—is a strong risk factor for ASD, the cause of this sex bias is not yet well understood. The “extreme male brain” theory suggests that the male sex bias may reflect the role of male sex hormones (i.e., androgens) in the etiology of ASD; specifically, risk for ASD may stem from increased androgen (e.g., testosterone) exposure during critical periods of fetal development, as the brain undergoes substantial organization and sexual differentiation [3, 4]. Indeed, numerous studies have found that greater prenatal androgen exposure is associated with ASD and ASD-related traits [58], although other studies have observed no relationship [9, 10]. Postnatal studies utilizing clinical and community samples of adults have also identified links between testosterone levels and ASD-related traits [11, 12], although postnatal studies of children show mixed results [13, 14]. Interestingly, despite evidence from many of these studies that greater testosterone is associated with phenotypes that characterize neurodevelopmental disorders more broadly—for instance, poor language abilities [15, 16]—the majority of studies that examine androgen exposure focus on ASD. Given the mixed evidence and dearth of studies examining NDDs outside of ASD, a better understanding of whether and how testosterone exposure may contribute to the male bias observed across all NDDs is warranted [8].
The links between androgen exposure and neurodevelopment have primarily been established via cross-sectional studies that measure testosterone levels at a single time point during perinatal (e.g., amniotic fluid, umbilical cord blood) or postnatal (e.g., blood serum, saliva) development. Beyond the practical challenges of measuring testosterone via biological samples, particularly during the perinatal period, these measures provide only a snapshot of androgen exposure at the time of collection, rather than capturing an individual’s cumulative exposure across development. To overcome the limitations of measuring testosterone levels directly, morphological and genetic measures are increasingly being leveraged to indirectly assess androgen-related risk. Digit ratio, or the ratio in length of the second and fourth digit (i.e., 2D:4D ratio), is a sexually dimorphic feature that is lower in males than in females [17] and has been linked to fetal testosterone exposure [18]. Numerous studies indicate that individuals with ASD exhibit lower (i.e., more masculine) digit ratios than their typically developing counterparts [1923], and evidence suggests this may be true for other NDDs as well [2426]. However, a number of recent studies have failed to replicate the association between fetal testosterone exposure and digit ratio [27, 28], indicating that digit ratio may be a useful biomarker of NDDs, but its biological underpinnings require further clarification.
Facial masculinity has also been proposed as a potential indicator of fetal testosterone and NDD-related risk. A twin study found that females with a male twin, who were presumably exposed to greater fetal testosterone levels, exhibited greater facial masculinity than those with a female twin [29] and another study showed that greater umbilical cord testosterone levels were associated with greater adult facial masculinity in both sexes [30]. Moreover, recent studies have found that facial masculinity predicts ASD traits in a variety of populations including children with ASD [31], siblings of individuals with ASD [32], as well as non-clinical samples [33, 34]. However, these studies provide inconsistent support for the “extreme male brain” theory [35, 36]. While some studies found that greater masculinity was associated with greater ASD traits in both sexes, others suggested that androgynous features may confer greater risk. For example, one study found that female subjects with ASD demonstrated more masculine faces than their typically developing peers, but males with ASD exhibited less masculine faces than their peers [36]. Given that these early findings are mixed, and that a dearth of studies has examined facial masculinity in the context of other NDDs, it remains unclear whether facial masculinity can yield insight into NDDs and, if so, how it may reflect androgen-related risk.
Running parallel to morphological proxies of androgen exposure, genetic studies of NDDs can yield insight into the hypothesized role of androgens in the etiology of NDDs. While large-scale studies have so far not identified individual genomic loci that strongly implicate sex hormone pathways as a major risk factor, some smaller studies have yielded clues worthy of further investigation. For example, one study (n = 174) found that three genes (the estrogen receptor ESR2 and two steroid enzymes, CYP11B1 and CYP17A1) related to the synthesis, transport, or metabolism of sex hormones were associated with ASD-related traits in non-clinical samples [37]. Another study found that CYP19A1, a steroid enzyme that contributes to sexual differentiation of the brain, was associated with dyslexia, a form of language impairment [38]. Yet another (n = 1171) identified three genes—that encoded an estrogen receptor (ESR1), steroid enzyme (SRD5A2), and sex-hormone binding globulin (SHBG)—as conferring risk for ASD-related traits in males [39], although replication in a larger sample (n = 10,654) found that only variation in SHBG emerged as a significant predictor [40]. Interestingly, levels of SHBG are inversely associated with levels of active (i.e., “free”) testosterone. Testosterone becomes inactive when it is bound to SHBG, and studies show that low levels of SHBG are found in individuals with excessive androgen activity [41]. These findings suggest that SHBG and other hormone-binding proteins may play an important role in the etiology of ASD through regulation of testosterone exposure. Moreover, given evidence that SHBG and free testosterone levels have been linked to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [42] and that genome-wide studies have identified significant genetic overlap between ASD and other NDDs [43, 44], it is possible that variation in SHBG and other testosterone regulators may contribute to NDDs more broadly. Further investigation of these potential mechanisms is needed.
Together, these previous studies point to several important yet unanswered questions. First, which morphological proxies of androgen exposure are most predictive of NDD diagnosis? Second, what specific behavioral and cognitive phenotypes are most related to these proxies of androgen exposure? Third, do morphological proxies such as digit ratio and facial morphology capture the same or different molecular aspects of masculinization, and how do these mechanisms relate to neurodevelopmental risk? Finally, how do quantitative effects related to androgen exposure in both sexes compare to the binary sex effect of being a Y-chromosomal male? The answers to these questions will deliver vital insight into the nature of male bias in NDDs. Toward this end, we carried out an investigation in two steps (Fig. 1). First, we assembled a neurodevelopmental cohort (N = 763) that includes (1) genome-wide genotypes, (2) measures of digit ratio masculinity (DRM), (3) measures of facial landmark masculinity (FLM) derived by machine learning, and (4) extensive clinical and parent-reported phenotypic information. This is the first study to investigate genetic factors associated with both digital and facial masculinity and their connection to neurodevelopmental outcomes. Second, to ensure the robustness of findings generated by investigation of our discovery cohort, we employed SPARK, a large genetic study of autism (N = 9419 in this analysis), as a means for testing the generalization of our observations.

Methods

Sample: devGenes

devGenes is a neurodevelopmental registry drawing participants largely from the midwestern United States and Iowa in particular. From 2015 to 2020, individuals of any age with a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), intellectual disability (ID) , language impairment, and/or pediatric epilepsy (including individuals with a syndromic form of NDD [n = 15; 2.0% of total sample, see Additional file 1: Table 1]) were recruited to participate in a genetic study of neurodevelopmental disorders. Biological relatives, regardless of age or affected status, were invited to participate as well. As such, the devGenes sample is enriched for individuals with NDDs but includes participants across a broad range of diagnoses and ages (i.e., 2–80 years; Table 1).
Table 1
Demographics (devGenes)
Name
Male (N = 381)
Female (N = 382)
N/mean
Percent/± SD
N/mean
Percent/± SD
Age
21.31
17.45
30
18.15
Caucasian
264
83.3%
273
85.6%
ASD
135
35.40%
46
12.00%
ADHD
96
25.20%
52
13.60%
ID
97
25.50%
36
9.40%
Epilepsy
12
3.10%
8
2.10%
Language impairment
112
29.40%
44
11.50%
Depression
19
5.00%
36
9.40%
Bipolar disorder
4
1.00%
9
2.40%
Anxiety
28
7.30%
34
8.90%
Unaffected
165
43.30%
253
66.20%
Genetic syndromes
10
66.67%
5
33.33%
Participants were informed of the study through a combination of public flyers, personal letters, follow-up phone calls, and local clinicians or community leaders. Participants met with a trained member of the research team for a single 60-minute visit in individuals’ homes, at independent and state-funded clinics, and in research laboratories at the University of Iowa. Study procedures were approved by the University of Iowa’s Institutional Review Board, and informed consent was obtained for each participant (IRB 201505743).

Sample: SPARK

SPARK is a nationwide (U.S.) genetic study of autism funded by the Simons Foundation [45]. Genetic and self- and parent-reported demographic and phenotypic data are made available to researchers through https://​base.​sfari.​org. We used the available genome-wide common variant genotyping data (described below) as well as results from the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) [46], SPARK version 4 phenotype release (Table 2).
Table 2
Demographics (SPARK)
Name
Male (N = 6802)
Female (N = 2596)
N/mean
Percent/± SD
N/mean
Percent/± SD
Age
8.07
4.09
7.95
4.32
Caucasian
4177
61.41%
1099
42.33%
ASD
5593
82.22%
1398
53.85%
ADHD
2150
31.61%
565
21.76%
ID
959
14.10%
307
11.83%
Epilepsy
246
3.62%
98
3.78%
Language impairment
3165
46.53%
800
30.82%
Depression
301
4.43%
125
4.82%
Bipolar disorder
77
1.13 %
33
1.27%
Anxiety
1012
14.88%
395
15.22%
Unaffected
1198
17.61%
1209
46.57%

Diagnostic and parent-report data

Participants in devGenes or their legal guardians were asked to complete a detailed survey regarding the participant’s demographics, developmental history, neurodevelopmental diagnoses (i.e., ADHD, ASD, language impairment (including dyslexia), and pediatric epilepsy), and lifetime mood/anxiety diagnoses (i.e., major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, and generalized anxiety). A total of \(N=239\) participants had parent-report data from 38 items in total (mean item missingness: 1.5%). Parent-report survey items were arranged by thematic group and their reliability assessed by Cronbach’s alpha (Additional file 1: Table 2). After survey completion, reported medical history and diagnoses were confirmed where possible by a member of the research team through examination of participants’ University of Iowa Hospital and Clinics medical records. All devGenes data were stored in a REDCap database [47, 48]. devGenes participants were classified as “affected” for analysis purposes if they reported any of the following diagnoses: ADHD, autism, intellectual disability, language impairment, or pediatric epilepsy. Participants were classified as “unaffected” if they did not endorse any of the five NDD diagnoses, even they reported a mood/anxiety diagnosis. In order to capture overall diagnostic burden, the total number of reported diagnoses (including both NDDs and mood/anxiety disorders) were summed for each participant, with levels of 0, 1, 2, and 3 or more. Basic individual-level data for devGenes participants are included in Additional file 1: Table 3, and we have made code available at https://​research-git.​uiowa.​edu/​michaelson-lab-public/​facial-masculinity-2021 to reproduce the analyses presented in Figs. 2, 3 and 4.

Masculinity measures

Hand scans/digit ratio masculinity (DRM)

Hand scans from devGenes participants were acquired using a flatbed image scanner operated by a trained research assistant. Participants were instructed to place both hands on the scanner, fingers spread and fully extended, to obtain a single scan of all ten fingers. The lengths of index fingers (i.e., second digits [2D]) and ring fingers (i.e., fourth digits [4D]) on both hands were measured manually using Image-J [49] (Fig. 2a).
Digit lengths were then corrected for rater and scanner effects with a linear model before being used to calculate left and right 2D:4D digit ratio. Using unaffected males and females, a lowess curve was fit that defined the trend, for each sex separately, of 2D:4D ratio with respect to age. To determine the 95% confidence interval of this trend, we performed bootstrap resampling 1000 times, each time recording the lowess curve (Fig. 2b). The mean curve of these bootstrap samples was then used as the point of reference, and all individual data points for either males or females were transformed to Z-statistics using the mean curve for that sex, and point estimates of the standard deviation. Finally, we multiplied these Z-statistics by \(-\,1\) so that increasingly positive values corresponded to increasing digit ratio masculinity. These Z-statistics (N = 664) were used in all subsequent analyses as “digit ratio masculinity” (DRM).

Two-dimensional facial photography/facial landmark masculinity (FLM)

Facial images from devGenes participants were acquired using a Nikon D3000 camera operated by a trained research assistant. Participants were instructed to look directly at the camera and maintain a neutral facial expression with closed lips (Fig. 2e).
Facial masculinity was assessed through computational analysis of the two-dimensional facial photographs. Coordinates (x, y) of 68 points (see Additional file 2: Fig. 1A) were extracted using dlib [50], a computer vision library that contains algorithms for machine learning, computer vision, and image processing, including facial landmark detection. We validated this approach by comparing it with manual annotation of a provisional set of 17 facial landmarks on a subset of 99 pictures, in which the landmarks were manually selected to represent the overall facial structure with less redundancy. Results from this analysis indicated significant concordance overlap of coordinates between manual annotations and dlib (average Spearman correlation: 0.962 ; 95%CI [0.959, 0.966]). For the present study, we used a subset of 12 coordinates (Fig. 2a), given the redundancy of landmarks within very close proximity and large variation of points around the jaw and chin region (Spearman correlation < 0.9). Landmark coordinates were normalized to a [0, 1] interval in both x and y directions, as facial photographs tend to have different dimensions. The Euclidean distance between landmark pairs coordinates was then computed and used to provide 66 distance features [51].
A Random Forest classifier [52] was then trained to discriminate males from females using these 66 features, as well as age in years (67 features total). Stratified sampling (via the strata and sampsize arguments to the randomForest function [53]) was performed so that all eight combinations of sex, age (dichotomized at 18 years), and affected status were approximately balanced at N = 75 for each of the eight groups, for each tree of the Random Forest. The ntree parameter was set to 5000 and mtry was set to 2 to reduce the greediness of the learning and increase diversity among the trees. Out-of bag estimates of misclassification showed a 38 % misclassification rate for females and a 33 % misclassification rate for males. The proportion of votes for class: male was used as the measure of masculinity. This vote proportion was transformed with a logistic function that put the midpoint of the curve at 0.4, which represents the vote proportion threshold that maximized the out-of-bag sensitivity and specificity, as determined by the F measure. These recalibrated class probabilities were then mapped onto a standard normal distribution. Next, to both observe and correct for the effects of age and sex on this masculinity estimate, we used unaffected males and females to fit a lowess curve that defined the trend, for each sex separately, of facial masculinity with respect to age. To determine the 95% confidence interval of this trend, we performed bootstrap resampling 1,000 times, each time recording the lowess curve (Fig. 2f). The mean curve of these bootstrap samples was then used as the point of reference, and all individual data points for either males or females were transformed to Z-statistics using the mean curve for that sex, and point estimates of the standard deviation. These Z-statistics (N = 747) were used in all subsequent analyses as “facial landmark masculinity” (FLM). Individual-level FLM and DRM measures are included in Additional file 1: Table 3.

Genotyping and imputation (devGenes)

DNA was extracted from saliva using the Autogen QuickGene-610L kit (www.​autogen.​com, Catalog #FK-DBLC). The DNA quantity was assessed using the Qubit dsDNA High Sensitivity assay kit (www.​thermofisher.​com, Catalog #Q32854). The mean DNA concentration was 65.5 ng\(\backslash \upmu {\mathrm{L}}\). The DNA quality was assessed using 1% agarose gels. All samples were genotyped using Illumina Infinium PsychArray microarrays in two batches, the first using Illumina PsychArray v1.1, the second using PsychArray v1.3.
SNPs were mapped to the hg19 reference of the human genome. All Quality Control (QC) steps carried out with PLINK [54] and R [55], based on QC process described in [56]. R was used for calculating standard deviations from heterozygosity and population outliers. All other QC was carried out in PLINK. Samples and SNPs with high global missing rate were removed. This was carried out in two stages so that highly problematic SNPs, not assayed with acceptable confidence rate, and individuals do not cause systemic problems at the more stringent threshold. First, four samples and 20,307 SNPs with missing rates above 20% were removed. Second, 17 samples and 7255 SNPs with missing rates above 5% were removed. Then, 233,882 SNPs with very low minor allele frequency (MAF), smaller than 1% in the cohort, were removed. 42,950 SNPs which grossly defy Hardy–Weinberg principle (HWE), HWE p-value smaller than 1e\(-\)10, were removed. Samples with missing rate higher than 5% on any one autosome would have been removed, but none met this criterion. Nine samples with extreme heterozygosity rates, more than 3 standard deviations from the mean rate, were removed. Six samples that were outliers on the first 10 components of multidimensional scaling, used to determine population structure, were removed. This effectively removed samples with ethnic backgrounds not captured in 1,000 Genomes [57], or more subtle admixture. The final number of samples used from devGenes was N = 239, for 295,362 SNPs.
After QC, samples were clustered based on genotype to identify population substructure of the sample. The cohort was merged with the samples from 1,000 Genomes phase 3. The combined cohort was clustered into 5 groups, representing the 5 distinct super-populations found in 1,000 Genomes samples. Clustering was based on the first 10 components from multi-dimensional scaling of the combined kinship matrix of the cohort and 1,000 Genomes samples. The top 20 principal components of each of the 5 clusters of the cohort were calculated separately. These components were used in downstream analyses to correct for population substructure. Samples which clustered with the European 1,000 Genomes samples were used for all subsequent analyses. Samples and SNPs which passed QC were imputed to the 1,000 Genomes phase 3 reference using the genipe pipeline [58]. Further, imputed genotype calls were quality filtered based on the default parameters of the genipe pipeline [58]. Individual imputed genotypes with a probability less than 90% were set to missing. Imputed SNPs with more than 2% missing calls were excluded. Moreover, imputed sites with minor allele frequency in the full cohort of less 1% were excluded. LD was calculated and files were handled with PLINK [54]. Phasing of genotypes was done with SHAPEIT [59]. Imputation was performed by IMPUTE2 [60].

Genotyping and imputation (SPARK)

Array genotyping data were generated and processed by SPARK (Freeze three, 2019-09-12), see original publication [61] for details. SNPs were mapped to the hg19 reference of the human genome. Mapping was done with liftOver, from hg38 to hg19. All QC steps carried out with PLINK [54] and R [55], based on the QC process described in [56]. R was used for calculating standard deviations from heterozygosity and population outliers.
Samples and SNPs with missing rates above 20% were removed, then samples and SNPs with missing rates above 5% were removed. This happens in two stages so that highly problematic SNPs and individuals do not cause systemic problems at the more stringent threshold. SNPs with MAF \(< 1\%\) in the cohort were removed, along with SNPs with a HWE p-value \(< 1{\mathrm{e}}-10\). Sample were removed with missing rate > 5% on any one autosome. Samples with heterozygosity rates more than 3 standard deviations from the mean rate were removed. Samples with more than 3 standard deviations from the mean on any of the first 10 MDS components were removed. Also, samples with ethnic backgrounds not captured in 1,000 Genomes, or more subtle admixture were removed.
After QC, samples were clustered based on genotype to be assigned to identify population substructure of the sample. The cohort was merged with the samples from the 1,000 Genomes phase 3 data. The combined cohort was clustered into 5 groups, representing the 5 distinct super-populations found in 1,000 Genomes. Clustering was based on the first 10 components from multi-dimensional scaling of the combined kinship matrix of the cohort and 1,000 Genomes. The top 20 principal components of each of the 5 clusters of the cohort were calculated separately. These components were used in downstream analyses to correct for population substructure. Samples which clustered with the 1,000 Genomes Europeans were used for all subsequent analyses. In total, 194,410 SNPs were removed (out of 616,321 SNPs), along with 1,922 individuals removed due to QC filters.
Samples and SNPs which passed QC were imputed to the 1,000 Genomes phase 3 reference using the genipe pipeline [58]. In brief, genipe performs LD calculation and file handling with PLINK [54], phasing of genotypes done with SHAPEIT [59], and imputation by IMPUTE2 [60].

Factor analysis

Factor analyses were performed using the factanal function in R, and fits ranging from 2 to 15 factors were attempted and characterized post hoc for fit and utility characteristics. For the devGenes parent report survey, a model of 11 factors was selected as a compromise between goodness-of-fit measures (which suggested a model with 10 factors was sufficient) and a jump in correlation with the masculinity measures that occurred at 11 factors (see Additional file 2: Fig. 2). The loadings of factors 1 through 11 suggest the following interpretations: (1) academic and learning deficits, (2) sensory issues and need for order, (3) aggression, (4) feeding issues, (5) self-harm, (6) stimming, (7) social anxiety, (8) cutting, (9) low social functioning, (10) gastrointestinal issues, (11) sensitivity to sound. For the SCQ in SPARK, no fit up to 15 factors suggested a sufficient number of factors by the Chi-squared test. We selected a factor model with eight factors as a compromise between interpretability and adequate capture of variance (\(\sim 50\%\)). The SCQ factors yielded from this analysis correspond approximately to joint attention (factor 1), odd behaviors (factor 2), odd speech (factor 3), nod-yes/shake-no (factor 4), appropriate facial expressions (factor 5), interest in other children (factor 6), makes conversation (factor 7), and cooperative/imaginative play (factor 8). See Fig. 5b for factor loadings. We further set aside SCQ items with uniqueness \(> 50\%\) (Additional file 2: Fig. 4) for consideration separate from the factors, since they were not adequately captured by the model.

Analysis of polygenic estimates

In order to investigate the underlying genetic factors associated with masculine features, we tested the association of masculinity (digit ratio and facial masculinity, in separate models) with polygenic risk scores relevant to autism, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, educational attainment, cognitive performance, testosterone (UK Biobank field ID 30850), sex-hormone binding globulin (SHBG, UK Biobank field ID 30830), and social dissatisfaction (UK Biobank field ID 4570, here coded such that higher values correspond to increasing dissatisfaction). Briefly, a polygenic risk score (PRS) is a sum of trait-associated alleles across many genetic loci, typically weighted by effect sizes estimated from a genome-wide association study (GWAS), which can act as a biomarker for a phenotype [62]. GWAS summary statistics for the traits noted above were downloaded from the UK Biobank [6365], Psychiatric Genomics Consortium [6668], and Social Science Genetic Association Consortium [69]. PRS for all individuals were calculated using the PRSice tool [70]. We evaluated two SNP inclusion thresholds: \(p<0.01\) and \(p<0.1\). To test PRS-trait associations, a linear model was fit to predict the outcome with PRS as an explanatory variable (each PRS was tested independently unless otherwise stated), with sex, age, and the top five genetic principal components as covariates.

Results

Study 1: devGenes

Facial landmark masculinity shows more pronounced sex and diagnostic differences than 2D:4D ratio

We examined the trends, across age, of both 2D:4D ratio and FLM within the entire (cross-sectional) devGenes cohort (Fig. 2). For 2D:4D ratio (Fig. 2a), we first examined the trends of unaffected (i.e., no reported NDD diagnosis) males and females (Fig. 2b). The 95% confidence intervals (CI) for unaffected males and females overlapped substantially until adulthood (30 years), which, although a more stringent criterion than significance testing, may suggest lower power to detect sex differences in child/adolescent samples. Comparing affected males to unaffected males (Fig. 2c), we observed that the affected trend exceeded the unaffected 95% CI for individuals in the 20s–40s age range. This suggests that affected adult males show more feminine 2D:4D ratios than their unaffected counterparts. Considering the 2D:4D ratio of affected females (Fig. 2d), we observed that the trend likewise showed a tendency toward greater femininity than unaffected females. In both cases, the null hypothesis suggested by the extreme male brain theory of \(M_{NDD} \leqslant M_{TD}\) (where M is some objective measure of masculinity) could not be rejected using the digit ratio data (empirical \(p=0.988\) for males and \(p=0.999\) for females; see Additional file 2: Fig. 4).
Turning to facial masculinity (Fig. 2e), we observed a more pronounced separation of the unaffected males and females, with complete separation of 95% CIs by age 13 years (Fig. 2f). In contrast to 2D:4D ratio, we observed that affected males appeared more masculine by facial landmarks than unaffected males, with the affected trend staying outside the 95% CI for individuals as old as 60 (Fig. 2g). Similarly, we observed that affected females displayed greater facial masculinity than their unaffected counterparts (Fig. 2h), again with the affected trend remaining outside the 95% CI even for individuals of older ages, where the sample contained fewer data points. In contrast to 2D:4D ratio, data from facial masculinity could reject the null hypothesis suggested by the extreme male brain theory of \(M_{NDD} \leqslant M_{TD}\) (empirical \(p=0.002\) for males and \(p<0.001\) for females; see Additional file 2: Fig. 4). Based on these observed differences across ages and sexes, all subsequent analyses used age- and sex-corrected measures of 2D:4D ratio (digit ratio masculinity or DRM) and facial landmark masculinity (FLM). We found these measures of DRM and FLM to be nominally correlated (\(r=0.081, p=0.037\)).

FLM is associated with NDD diagnoses

Having become familiar with the basic patterns of 2D:4D ratio and facial masculinity in the same neurodevelopmental cohort, we next asked whether these morphological proxies for androgen exposure were significantly associated with specific diagnoses. Using t-tests, we compared the age- and sex-adjusted DRM and FLM scores in each diagnostic group (ADHD, ASD, ID, language disorder, epilepsy, depression, bipolar disorder, and anxiety disorder) with those who lacked these diagnoses (Fig. 3). We found that DRM was not significantly associated with any NDD diagnoses (all \(p>0.05\)) and was nominally and negatively associated with anxiety (\(p=0.04; t=-2.15, {\text{FDR}}=0.21\), Fig. 3a), while FLM was significantly and positively associated with ADHD (\(p=0.016; t=2.42\)), ASD (\(p=0.01, t=2.6\)), and ID (\(p=0.006, t=2.76\)) even after correction for multiple testing (all \({\text{FDR}}<0.05\), Fig. 3b). Language disorder and epilepsy achieved nominal significance (\(p<0.05, {\text{FDR}}>0.05\)). Taken together, these results suggest that in the devGenes cohort, increasing FLM is associated with NDDs broadly and not exclusively with any particular NDD.

DRM and FLM are associated with parent-reported concerns about social functioning

We next turned to parent-reported concerns across a variety of domains, including social deficits, restricted and repetitive behaviors, academic performance, language, sensory issues, aggression, self-harm, eating and gastrointestinal issues, and sleep. We combined these parent reported items into latent variables using factor analysis, which yielded 11 factors (Fig. 3c, see methods). We tested the association of (age and sex-adjusted) DRM and FLM with each factor (Fig. 3d) using the Spearman correlation. The only factor for which both DRM and FLM achieved a significant association (\(\rho =0.2, p=0.004\), FDR = 0.04 and \(\rho =0.19, p=0.004\), FDR = 0.04, respectively) was factor 9, which loads heavily on items where parents were asked about the frequency of social interaction with individuals outside the immediate family, and a qualitative descriptor of the number of friends the child has (none, a few, many). The items related to social functioning fell into two factors, one related to social anxiety (factor 7) and the other to social functioning (factor 9). Our measures of masculinity were only significantly associated with factor 9, which may suggest some specificity of androgen-related mechanisms for social functioning (extent and quality of friendships and social connections) rather than social anxiety (fear of social interactions). A follow-up analysis fit a linear model predicting the factor 9 score as a function of both DRM and FLM together. Both DRM and FLM were significantly associated with factor 9 (\(\beta =0.09, {\text{SE}}=0.04, t=2.15\), p = 0.03 and \(\beta =0.1, {\text{SE}}=0.046, t=2.17\), p = 0.03), suggesting independent and additive contributions of similar magnitude.

Polygenic association of social functioning, DRM, and FLM with testosterone and SHBG

With evidence that DRM and FLM might contribute additively to social deficits and potentially through different mechanisms, we next sought to improve our mechanistic and etiological insight into these traits by testing their association with a collection of polygenic risk scores (PRS) relevant to the topic of this investigation. We first examined parent-reported social interaction frequency and its associations with polygenic estimates of total serum testosterone (UK Biobank field ID 30850), sex hormone binding globulin (SHBG, UK Biobank field ID 30830), an item from the UK Biobank measuring satisfaction with friendships (UK Biobank field ID 4570, here coded such that higher values correspond to increasing dissatisfaction), ASD risk [68], ADHD risk [67], cognitive performance, and educational attainment [69] (Fig. 4). For DRM (Fig. 4a), we observed significant positive associations for testosterone and SHBG PRS (\(p<0.05\)), though only testosterone remained significant after adjustment for multiple testing (\(\beta =0.8\), \({\text{SE}}=0.2\), \(t=4.0\), \(p={8.8\times 10^{-5}}\), \({\text{FDR}}=0.001\)). For FLM (Fig. 4b), we observed a significant negative association for SHBG PRS and positive associations for social dissatisfaction PRS and cognitive performance PRS (all \(p<0.05\)), though only SHBG (\(\beta =-0.58, {\text{SE}}=0.18, t=-3.26, p=0.001\), FDR = 0.009) and cognitive performance (\(\beta =0.69, {\text{SE}}=0.19, t=3.57, p=0.0004\), FDR = 0.006) remained significant after adjustment for multiple testing. For factor 9 (the low social functioning factor, 4C), no PRS survived multiple testing correction (\({\text{FDR}}<0.05\)), though importantly both testosterone and SHBG PRS met or approached nominal significance (\(p<0.05\)) and in directions concordant with our working hypothesis about the net androgen exposure leading to reduced social functioning (\(\beta =0.27\pm .14\) and \(\beta =-0.41\pm 0.15\) for testosterone PRS and SHBG PRS, respectively). For factor 9, there was also a nominally significant negative association with ASD PRS (\(p<0.05\)), though this did not survive correction for multiple testing (\({\text{FDR}}>0.05\)). Using a SNP threshold of \(p<0.01\) for testosterone PRS and \(p<0.1\) for testosterone and SHBG PRS, respectively (which were the SNP thresholds where maximal association was observed in DRM and FLM), we visualized the PRS associations with each trait by binning PRS into the bottom 20%, middle 60%, and top 20% (Fig. 4d–i). This illustrates that DRM is most strongly associated with testosterone PRS, while FLM is only associated (negatively) with SHBG PRS. Social impairment (factor 9) is positively associated with testosterone PRS and negatively associated with SHBG PRS.

Study 2: SPARK

In study 1, results were consistent with the hypothesis that elevated testosterone and low SHBG (overall corresponding to more potent net androgen exposure) may contribute to the observed social deficits that were indexed by our morphological estimates of masculinity, DRM and FLM. In this replication study, with an order of magnitude more participants (N = 9419) than study 1, we investigated whether testosterone PRS and SHBG PRS would similarly predict indices of social functioning. We also compared the estimated effect of male sex (here a binary variable based on the presumed chromosomal sex of the participant) on these social functioning indices to the effect of testosterone PRS and SHBG PRS.

Replication in SPARK of testosterone and SHBG association with social functioning

To test the generalization of the hypothesis that emerged from our previous analyses, that is, that higher testosterone and lower SHBG levels might lead to deficits in social behavior, we used SPARK, a large, US-based nationwide genetic study of autism spectrum disorders. In addition to genetic data, SPARK has a host of phenotypic data relevant to ASD. Given our working hypothesis relating to social deficits, we focused on the results of the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) [46], which has 40 items designed to ascertain social communication issues typically seen in ASD. The SCQ was originally devised as a screen to use in binary clinical decision-making, though it has been used extensively in research. We again used factor analysis (see methods) to decompose the SCQ items into latent variables (eight factors, Fig. 5a, b). In addition, we performed item-level tests of association for SCQ items that were poorly captured in the factor model (see methods, Fig. 5c). Using bivariate linear or logistic regression models (polygenic estimates of testosterone and SHBG as explanatory variables), we tested association of PRS with indices of social function. Among the factors, only factor 8, which loads on items relating to cooperative and imaginative play, showed significant associations with male sex (\(\beta =-0.19, {\text{SE}}=0.02, t=-8.15, p={4.18\times 10^{-16}}\), FDR < 0.01), testosterone PRS (\(\beta =-0.03, {\text{SE}}=0.01, t=-2.34, p=0.02\), FDR = 0.16), and SHBG PRS (\(\beta =0.03, {\text{SE}}=0.01, t=2.74, p=0.006\), FDR =0.049). Notably, all associations were in the direction predicted by our working hypothesis (note that in this case, a higher score for factor 8 indicates more prosocial behavior). Among the individual items not well-captured by the factor model (Fig. 5c), only SCQ item 19 (“has a best friend or friends”) was significantly associated with male sex (\(\beta =-0.22, {\text{SE}}=0.05, Z=-4.15, p={3.27\times 10^{-05}}\), FDR < 0.01), testosterone PRS (\(\beta =-0.06, {\text{SE}}=0.03, Z=-2.31, p=0.02\), FDR = 0.31), and SHBG PRS (\(\beta =0.11, {\text{SE}}=0.03, Z=4.16, p={3.15\times 10^{-05}}\), FDR < 0.01). Again, the directions of these associations were in agreement with our working hypothesis (note that again, the direction is such that a higher value indicates prosocial behavior).

Comparing effect of binary sex to autosomal polygenic sex hormone effects

The effects noted for testosterone and SHBG PRS in the above linear and logistic regression models are on a “per-SD of PRS scale”. That is, for each increase of one standard deviation of PRS, there will be an expected increase (or decrease) in the trait of \(\beta \pm {\text{SE}}\). To meaningfully compare these PRS estimates to that of binary sex, which is an effect from one extreme of the variable (female or 0) to the other (male or 1), we must scale the PRS effects to likewise span their extremes. In this case, we choose the PRS values at the 0.1%-ile and the 99.9%-ile. The difference between these PRS values (which have already been scaled to mean 0 and SD 1) acts as a scaling factor for both the PRS \(\beta\) and its standard error. Using this approach, we find that the expected effect of binary male sex is comparable or even perhaps less extreme than the apparent effects of testosterone and SHBG PRS (factor 8: male sex: \(-0.19\pm 0.03\), testosterone PRS: \(-0.15\pm 0.066\), SHBG PRS: \(0.18\pm 0.066\), see Fig. 5d; SCQ item 19: male sex: \(-0.22\pm 0.05\) testosterone PRS: \(-0.35\pm 0.15\), SHBG PRS: \(0.64\pm 0.15\), see Fig. 5e).

Discussion

The current study employed a two-step approach to investigate genetic factors associated with both digital and facial masculinity and their connection to neurodevelopmental outcomes. In the first study, which was the first of its kind to examine both digit ratio and facial masculinity in the same genetically characterized neurodevelopmental cohort, we found convergent evidence that androgen exposure is associated with deficits in social functioning, even while correcting for binary sex. We found that facial landmark masculinity (FLM) was predictive of NDD diagnoses (ADHD, ASD, ID), in the direction consistent with the extreme male brain theory, while digit ratio masculinity (DRM) was not. The predictive power of FLM relative to DRM suggests that further pursuit of biomarkers and endophenotypes based on facial morphology may prove more fruitful than digit ratio. Particularly noteworthy is that social functioning showed convergent association with both FLM and DRM. Through analysis in connection with polygenic risk scores (PRS), our findings implicate both testosterone and sex hormone binding globulin (SHBG) levels as complementary and potentially additive factors that may impact social functioning. In the second study, our hypothesis found additional statistical support in the large SPARK cohort, and we found evidence suggesting that the autosomal genetic factors (i.e., the PRS) that predict testosterone and SHBG levels may exert effects on social functioning that are comparable in magnitude to the effect of binary (i.e., chromosomal) sex itself. This provides evidence that, at least in the context of its effect on social functioning, sex may be more accurately described as bimodal and continuous, rather than binary and discrete.
Notably, the direction of associations between FLM and NDD diagnosis was consistent with the extreme male brain theory (i.e., greater masculinity conferred greater risk for NDD traits), which was not the case for DRM. Although DRM was associated with social deficits in the expected direction, when considering clinical diagnoses, young- to middle-adulthood males with NDDs demonstrated more feminine digit ratios than their typically developing peers. This is not the first study to find that androgynous features are associated with NDDs [33] and that males with ASD demonstrate more feminine digit ratios [36]. However, quantitative reviews have supported the association between masculine digit ratio and ASD [71, 72], so the literature on DRM remains mixed. It is also unclear why the association between DRM and affected status did not emerge until young-adulthood, given that NDD-related outcomes emerge in early childhood. Longitudinal studies of typically developing children show that digit ratio tends to increase (i.e., becomes more feminine) across puberty [73, 74], however the rank order of inter-individual differences remain stable. No study to date has examined the developmental trajectory of DRM in individuals with NDDs, or longitudinally compared groups of individuals with and without NDDs, so further research in this area may be useful. Nevertheless, our results suggest that FLM may provide greater utility than DRM as a biomarker with clinical relevance.
The differential performance between FLM and DRM in predicting NDD traits may reflect a number of physiological factors that differentiate the two measures of masculinity. First, combining distances between 12 facial landmarks naturally provides a richer basis for an estimator of masculinity than the average length of two digits. As such, the face inherently provides a more sensitive indicator for subtle differences in biological risk. In contrast to digits, faces are more proximal to the brain not just physically, but in development and biology. Prenatal brain development is intimately tied to craniofacial development through a combination of biochemical mechanisms and genetic signaling [29]. Brain and craniofacial tissues originate from the same population of cells, such as neural crest cells, which differentiate early in fetal development to eventually form the brain and face [75]. As such, developmental perturbations in the brain are more likely to manifest in craniofacial features than digits [7678]. These developmental mechanisms support our findings that FLM is associated with NDDs broadly, and not only to ASD. Given the ties between neurodevelopment and craniofacial development, it follows that facial morphology is a useful basis for predicting neurodevelopmental risk generally, rather than ASD in particular. This was underscored by the convergence of DRM and FLM on a factor that loads on social functioning. That this convergence on social impairment was found in a mixed NDD cohort is noteworthy: although ASD is most prominently characterized by deficits in social functioning, other NDDs are also marked by elevated rates of social dysfunction [79, 80], underscoring the fact that key features of NDDs, and psychopathology more broadly, do not adhere to diagnostic boundaries [43, 44].
To provide potential mechanistic insights, we investigated the association of specific polygenic risk scores to DRM, FLM, and factor 9, a latent variable capturing social impairment which was found to be a point of convergence between FLM and DRM. Both DRM and FLM were significantly predicted by androgen-related PRS: DRM by testosterone and FLM by SHBG, an attenuator of free testosterone. These and other results of our investigations suggest that DRM and FLM may access a latent masculinity through complementary androgen-related mechanisms. Social impairment (factor 9) showed a combined nominal association where increasing testosterone PRS and decreasing SHBG PRS were independently and additively associated with increased social deficits. Taken together, these results pointed toward a hypothesis of increased net androgen exposure resulting in reduced social functioning.
Our investigation in the much larger SPARK cohort (N = 9419) added further evidence in favor of this hypothesis, as male sex, testosterone PRS, and SHBG PRS were all significantly associated, in the anticipated direction, with cooperative/imaginative play, and having a best friend or friends. It is noteworthy that these indicators emerged from a background of other possible social phenotypes, including joint attention, facial expressions, and binary communication (yes/no by head nodding or shaking). Our working hypothesis was based on the initial observation of a testosterone/SHBG association with a factor that, as in the SPARK study, loaded on friendships and social interactions (and not on other social phenotypes such as social anxiety). Further investigation is needed, but the fact that both the discovery sample and the replication sample converged on a friendship-oriented social phenotype is striking.
When considering the genomic reservoir of androgen-related risk, it seems intuitive that the sex chromosomes (and therefore our binary concept of male and female sex) would play a dominant role. However, our comparison of sex chromosome and autosomal effects (binary sex and polygenic estimates of testosterone and SHBG, respectively, see Fig. 5d, e) led to the unexpected finding that sex chromosome and autosomal contributions are comparable to each other in magnitude, with respect to risk of social deficits. Although perhaps not intuitive, this should not be surprising, given that a sizable majority of genes involved in androgen metabolism and sex differentiation pathways are encoded on autosomes (see Additional file 1: Table 5), and that genetic variation in these genes will shape and tune an individual’s response to the presence of sex hormones throughout their development. While further research is needed, these findings suggest the possibility of a latent and continuous “autosomal sex” that, like binary sex, plays a major role in neurodevelopmental outcomes, especially those related to social functioning.
How might the proposed genetic risk mechanisms impact social functioning? Sex hormones affect brain structure and function through multiple mechanisms and on different time scales. Human and animal research suggests that both short-term (e.g., neurotransmitter and neuropeptide) and long-term (i.e., developmental) mechanisms downstream of androgen exposure can lead to reduced capacity for social functioning [8184]. Studies in humans that have linked fetal testosterone exposure (FTE) to later reduced quality of social relationships [85] and reduced empathic descriptions of social interactions [86] underscore the long-term processes, potentially related to neural circuit formation, that may explain androgen-related social deficits. Taken together, the early predictive value of FTE and our observation that affected individuals show elevated masculinity relative to typically developing peers, even at an early age (Fig 2. panels g, h), suggest that indices of sex hormone exposure may hold promise as early markers of developmental risk. The more immediate behavioral effects of androgens might be explained by their interactions with signaling pathways involving the neuropeptides oxytocin and vasopressin, which have been well-established as key mechanisms in social behavior [87, 88], and have recently shown promise in clinical trials for their influence on social functioning [89]. Our results build on this previous work by providing genetic evidence that increasing testosterone and decreasing SHBG (overall corresponding to more potent net androgen exposure) may underlie the observed social deficits that were indexed by our morphological estimates of masculinity, DRM and FLM. Furthermore, our results may offer a partial explanation for why males are diagnosed with NDDs at high rates than females [1]: males, on average, will have higher androgen exposure and production throughout their lives, and are consequently at greater risk for disorders where social impairment is a factor in diagnosis, such as ASD and other NDDs. Conversely, females have lower androgen production and exposure throughout their lives, making this risk mechanism generally less applicable and resulting in a lower rate of diagnosis with disorders where social impairment is a decisive factor. A key contribution of this work is that we show that autosomal androgen-related risk mechanisms (shown here as polygenic risk scores) can and do operate in males and females alike, contributing risk above and beyond that linked to sex chromosomes.

Limitations

Despite the replication of our findings, our study has several limitations that are important to acknowledge when considering the trajectory of future work. First, we did not directly measure sex hormones, but instead used well-supported upstream (polygenic estimators) and downstream (morphological features) correlates as proxies for androgen exposure. While these are convenient to measure in large samples, further testing and refinement of hypotheses connecting genetics, sex hormones, and social functioning will require direct measurement of the relevant factors. Next, because our study was not interventional, the precise structure of causal relationships between sex hormones and social functioning remains unclear. Studies of short-term testosterone exposure have demonstrated significant effects on some behaviors such as risk taking [90] and aggression [91] but not others, such as empathizing [92]. Further interventional research across a range of exposure durations, for example in samples of patients undergoing hormone therapy [93], will be needed to further elucidate the mechanisms underlying the link between androgens and social functioning.

Conclusions

Across both males and females, increased masculinity—as indicated by morphological features (i.e., greater facial masculinity) and genetic factors (e.g., decreased SHBG polygenic score, which typically coincides with increased testosterone)—increases risk for NDD symptomatology, particularly impairments in social functioning. These findings not only align with prior evidence that increased androgen exposure plays a key role in the male bias observed in autism spectrum disorder and other NDDs, but also suggest that further research is needed to understand how nonbinary (e.g., autosomal) androgen-related factors contribute to differences in neurodevelopment and social functioning.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all devGenes participants for their important contributions to this research. We would also like to thank Atticus Michaelson for his permission to use his image in Fig. 2. We are grateful to all of the families in SPARK, the SPARK clinical sites and SPARK staff. We appreciate obtaining access to genetic and phenotypic data for SPARK data on SFARI Base. Approved researchers can obtain the SPARK population dataset described in this study by applying at https://​base.​sfari.​org.

Declarations

Study procedures were approved by the University of Iowa’s Institutional Review Board, and informed consent was obtained for each participant (IRB 201505743).
Assent was obtained from the subject and consent from the legal guardian for the likeness appearing in Fig. 2.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no financial or other competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Rutter M, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Using sex differences in psychopathology to study causal mechanisms: unifying issues and research strategies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44(8):1092–115.PubMedCrossRef Rutter M, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Using sex differences in psychopathology to study causal mechanisms: unifying issues and research strategies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44(8):1092–115.PubMedCrossRef
2.
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Lombardo M, Chakrabarti B. The extreme male brain theory of autism: the role of fetal androgens; Autism spectrum disorders. 2011. pp. 991–8. Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Lombardo M, Chakrabarti B. The extreme male brain theory of autism: the role of fetal androgens; Autism spectrum disorders. 2011. pp. 991–8.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen S, Lombardo M, Auyeung B, Ashwin E, Chakrabarti B, Knickmeyer R. Why are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in males? PLoS Biol. 2011;9:e1001081.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Baron-Cohen S, Lombardo M, Auyeung B, Ashwin E, Chakrabarti B, Knickmeyer R. Why are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in males? PLoS Biol. 2011;9:e1001081.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S. Topical review: fetal testosterone and sex differences in typical social development and in autism. J Child Neurol. 2006;21:825–45.PubMedCrossRef Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S. Topical review: fetal testosterone and sex differences in typical social development and in autism. J Child Neurol. 2006;21:825–45.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G, Hines M. Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behavior in girls and in boys. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:144–8.PubMedCrossRef Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G, Hines M. Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differentiated childhood behavior in girls and in boys. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:144–8.PubMedCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Schmidtova E, Lakatosova S, Celec P, Ficek A, Ostatníková D. Polymorphisms in genes involved in testosterone metabolism in Slovak autistic boys. The Endocrinologist. 2010;20:245–9.CrossRef Schmidtova E, Lakatosova S, Celec P, Ficek A, Ostatníková D. Polymorphisms in genes involved in testosterone metabolism in Slovak autistic boys. The Endocrinologist. 2010;20:245–9.CrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Nørgaard-Pedersen B, Hougaard D, Abdallah M, Melgaard L, Cohen A, Chakrabarti B, Ruta L, Lombardo M. Elevated fetal steroidogenic activity in autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;20:06. Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Nørgaard-Pedersen B, Hougaard D, Abdallah M, Melgaard L, Cohen A, Chakrabarti B, Ruta L, Lombardo M. Elevated fetal steroidogenic activity in autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;20:06.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Kung K, Spencer D, Pasterski V, Neufeld S, Glover V, O’Connor T, Hindmarsh P, Hughes I, Acerini C, Hines M. No relationship between prenatal androgen exposure and autistic traits: convergent evidence from studies of children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia and of amniotic testosterone concentrations in typically-developing children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2016;57:07.CrossRef Kung K, Spencer D, Pasterski V, Neufeld S, Glover V, O’Connor T, Hindmarsh P, Hughes I, Acerini C, Hines M. No relationship between prenatal androgen exposure and autistic traits: convergent evidence from studies of children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia and of amniotic testosterone concentrations in typically-developing children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2016;57:07.CrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitehouse A, Mattes E, Maybery M, Dissanayake C, Sawyer M, Standing R, Pennell C, Keelan J, Hickey M. Perinatal testosterone exposure and autistic-like traits in the general population: a longitudinal pregnancy-cohort study. J Neurodev Disord. 2012;4:25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Whitehouse A, Mattes E, Maybery M, Dissanayake C, Sawyer M, Standing R, Pennell C, Keelan J, Hickey M. Perinatal testosterone exposure and autistic-like traits in the general population: a longitudinal pregnancy-cohort study. J Neurodev Disord. 2012;4:25.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Schwarz E, Guest P, Rahmoune H, Wang L, Levin Y, Ingudomnukul E, Ruta L, Kent L, Spain M, Baron-Cohen S, Bahn S. Sex-specific serum biomarker patterns in adults with asperger syndrome. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;16:1213–20.PubMedCrossRef Schwarz E, Guest P, Rahmoune H, Wang L, Levin Y, Ingudomnukul E, Ruta L, Kent L, Spain M, Baron-Cohen S, Bahn S. Sex-specific serum biomarker patterns in adults with asperger syndrome. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;16:1213–20.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruta L, Ingudomnukul E, Taylor K, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S. Increased serum androstenedione in adults with autism spectrum conditions. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:1154–63.PubMedCrossRef Ruta L, Ingudomnukul E, Taylor K, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S. Increased serum androstenedione in adults with autism spectrum conditions. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2011;36:1154–63.PubMedCrossRef
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Kung K, Constantinescu M, Browne W, Noorderhaven R, Hines M. No relationship between early postnatal testosterone concentrations and autistic traits in 18 to 30-month-old children. Mol Autism. 2016;7:12.CrossRef Kung K, Constantinescu M, Browne W, Noorderhaven R, Hines M. No relationship between early postnatal testosterone concentrations and autistic traits in 18 to 30-month-old children. Mol Autism. 2016;7:12.CrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Croonenberghs J, Grieken S, Wauters A, van West D, Brouw L, Maes M, Deboutte D. Serum testosterone concentration in male autistic youngsters. Neuro Endocrinol Lett. 2010;31:483.PubMed Croonenberghs J, Grieken S, Wauters A, van West D, Brouw L, Maes M, Deboutte D. Serum testosterone concentration in male autistic youngsters. Neuro Endocrinol Lett. 2010;31:483.PubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitehouse A, Maybery M, Hart R, Mattes E, Newnham J, Sloboda D, Stanley F, Hickey M. Fetal androgen exposure and pragmatic language ability of girls in middle childhood: implications for the extreme male-brain theory of autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:1259–64.PubMedCrossRef Whitehouse A, Maybery M, Hart R, Mattes E, Newnham J, Sloboda D, Stanley F, Hickey M. Fetal androgen exposure and pragmatic language ability of girls in middle childhood: implications for the extreme male-brain theory of autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:1259–64.PubMedCrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitehouse A, Mattes E, Maybery M, Sawyer M, Jacoby P, Keelan J, Hickey M. Sex-specific associations between umbilical cord blood testosterone levels and language delay in early childhood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2012;53:726–34.CrossRef Whitehouse A, Mattes E, Maybery M, Sawyer M, Jacoby P, Keelan J, Hickey M. Sex-specific associations between umbilical cord blood testosterone levels and language delay in early childhood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2012;53:726–34.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Manning J, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones D. The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Hum Reprod (Oxford, England). 1998;13:3000–4.CrossRef Manning J, Scutt D, Wilson J, Lewis-Jones D. The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Hum Reprod (Oxford, England). 1998;13:3000–4.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning J. 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Hum Dev. 2004;77:23–8.PubMedCrossRef Lutchmaya S, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Knickmeyer R, Manning J. 2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Hum Dev. 2004;77:23–8.PubMedCrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Manning J, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Sanders G. The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001;43:160–4.PubMedCrossRef Manning J, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Sanders G. The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001;43:160–4.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Bruin E, Verheij F, Wiegman T, Ferdinand R. Differences in finger length ratio between males with autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, adhd, and anxiety disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:962–5.PubMedCrossRef Bruin E, Verheij F, Wiegman T, Ferdinand R. Differences in finger length ratio between males with autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, adhd, and anxiety disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:962–5.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Milne E, White S, Campbell R, Swettenham J, Hansen P, Ramus F. Motion and form coherence detection in autistic spectrum disorder: Relationship to motor control and 2:4 digit ratio. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:225–37.PubMedCrossRef Milne E, White S, Campbell R, Swettenham J, Hansen P, Ramus F. Motion and form coherence detection in autistic spectrum disorder: Relationship to motor control and 2:4 digit ratio. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:225–37.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Noipayak P. The ratio of 2nd and 4th digit length in autistic children. J Med Assoc Thail Chotmaihet thangphaet. 2009;92:1040–5. Noipayak P. The ratio of 2nd and 4th digit length in autistic children. J Med Assoc Thail Chotmaihet thangphaet. 2009;92:1040–5.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Krajmer P, Špajdel M, Kubranská A, Ostatníková D. 2d:4d finger ratio in Slovak autism spectrum population. Bratislavské lekárske listy. 2011;112:377–9.PubMed Krajmer P, Špajdel M, Kubranská A, Ostatníková D. 2d:4d finger ratio in Slovak autism spectrum population. Bratislavské lekárske listy. 2011;112:377–9.PubMed
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Martel M. Conscientiousness as a mediator of the association between masculinized finger-length ratios and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (adhd). J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2009;50:790–8.CrossRef Martel M. Conscientiousness as a mediator of the association between masculinized finger-length ratios and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (adhd). J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2009;50:790–8.CrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Williams J, Greenhalgh K, Manning J. Second to fourth ratio and possible precursors of developmental psychopathology in preschool children. Early Hum Dev. 2003;72:57–65.PubMedCrossRef Williams J, Greenhalgh K, Manning J. Second to fourth ratio and possible precursors of developmental psychopathology in preschool children. Early Hum Dev. 2003;72:57–65.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Martel M, Gobrogge K, Breedlove S, Nigg J. Masculinized finger-length ratios of boys, but not girls, are associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Nurosci. 2008;122:273–81.CrossRef Martel M, Gobrogge K, Breedlove S, Nigg J. Masculinized finger-length ratios of boys, but not girls, are associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Nurosci. 2008;122:273–81.CrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Lawson L, Keelan J, Jamnadass E, Maybery M, Hickey M, Whitehouse A. Adult digit ratio (2d:4d) is not related to umbilical cord androgen or estrogen concentrations, their ratios or net bioactivity. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91:02. Lawson L, Keelan J, Jamnadass E, Maybery M, Hickey M, Whitehouse A. Adult digit ratio (2d:4d) is not related to umbilical cord androgen or estrogen concentrations, their ratios or net bioactivity. Early Hum Dev. 2015;91:02.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Hickey M, Doherty D, Hart R, Norman R, Mattes E, Atkinson H, Sloboda D. Maternal and umbilical cord androgen concentrations do not predict digit ratio (2d:4d) in girls: a prospective cohort study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:1235–44.PubMedCrossRef Hickey M, Doherty D, Hart R, Norman R, Mattes E, Atkinson H, Sloboda D. Maternal and umbilical cord androgen concentrations do not predict digit ratio (2d:4d) in girls: a prospective cohort study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:1235–44.PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Marečková K, Chakravarty M, Lawrence C, Leonard G, Pérusse D, Perron M, Pike B, Richer L, Veillette S, Pausova Z, Paus T. Identifying craniofacial features associated with prenatal exposure to androgens and testing their relationship with brain development. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;220:07. Marečková K, Chakravarty M, Lawrence C, Leonard G, Pérusse D, Perron M, Pike B, Richer L, Veillette S, Pausova Z, Paus T. Identifying craniofacial features associated with prenatal exposure to androgens and testing their relationship with brain development. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;220:07.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Whitehouse A, Gilani SZ, Shafait F, Mian A, Tan D, Maybery M, Keelan J, Hart R, Handelsman D, Goonawardene M. Prenatal testosterone exposure is related to sexually dimorphic facial morphology in adulthood. Proc R Soc B. 2015;282:20151351.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Whitehouse A, Gilani SZ, Shafait F, Mian A, Tan D, Maybery M, Keelan J, Hart R, Handelsman D, Goonawardene M. Prenatal testosterone exposure is related to sexually dimorphic facial morphology in adulthood. Proc R Soc B. 2015;282:20151351.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Tan D, Gilani SZ, Maybery M, Mian A, Hunt A, Walters M, Whitehouse A. Hypermasculinised facial morphology in boys and girls with autism spectrum disorder and its association with symptomatology. Sci Rep. 2017;7:9348.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tan D, Gilani SZ, Maybery M, Mian A, Hunt A, Walters M, Whitehouse A. Hypermasculinised facial morphology in boys and girls with autism spectrum disorder and its association with symptomatology. Sci Rep. 2017;7:9348.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Tan D, Maybery M, Gilani SZ, Alvares G, Mian A, Suter D, Whitehouse A. A broad autism phenotype expressed in facial morphology. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:01.CrossRef Tan D, Maybery M, Gilani SZ, Alvares G, Mian A, Suter D, Whitehouse A. A broad autism phenotype expressed in facial morphology. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:01.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Gilani SZ, Tan D, Russell-Smith S, Maybery M, Mian A, Eastwood P, Shafait F, Goonewardene M, Whitehouse A. Sexually dimorphic facial features vary according to level of autistic-like traits in the general population. J Neurodev Disord. 2015;7:12.CrossRef Gilani SZ, Tan D, Russell-Smith S, Maybery M, Mian A, Eastwood P, Shafait F, Goonewardene M, Whitehouse A. Sexually dimorphic facial features vary according to level of autistic-like traits in the general population. J Neurodev Disord. 2015;7:12.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Tan D, Russell-Smith S, Simons J, Maybery M, Leung D, Ng H, Whitehouse A. Perceived gender ratings for high and low scorers on the autism-spectrum quotient consistent with the extreme male brain account of autism. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:1.CrossRef Tan D, Russell-Smith S, Simons J, Maybery M, Leung D, Ng H, Whitehouse A. Perceived gender ratings for high and low scorers on the autism-spectrum quotient consistent with the extreme male brain account of autism. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:1.CrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Tan D, Maybery M, Ewing L, Tay J-X, Eastwood P, Whitehouse A. Sex-specific variation in facial masculinity/femininity associated with autistic traits in the general population. Br J Psychol. 2019;12:723–41. Tan D, Maybery M, Ewing L, Tay J-X, Eastwood P, Whitehouse A. Sex-specific variation in facial masculinity/femininity associated with autistic traits in the general population. Br J Psychol. 2019;12:723–41.
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Bejerot S, Eriksson J, Bonde S, Carlström K, Humble M, Eriksson E. The extreme male brain revisited: gender coherence in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci. 2012;201:116–23.CrossRef Bejerot S, Eriksson J, Bonde S, Carlström K, Humble M, Eriksson E. The extreme male brain revisited: gender coherence in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci. 2012;201:116–23.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Chakrabarti B, Dudbridge F, Kent L, Wheelwright S, Hill-Cawthorne G, Allison C, Banerjee-Basu S, Baron-Cohen S. Genes related to sex steroids, neural growth, and social-emotional behavior are associated with autistic traits, empathy, and asperger syndrome. Autism Res. 2009;2:06.CrossRef Chakrabarti B, Dudbridge F, Kent L, Wheelwright S, Hill-Cawthorne G, Allison C, Banerjee-Basu S, Baron-Cohen S. Genes related to sex steroids, neural growth, and social-emotional behavior are associated with autistic traits, empathy, and asperger syndrome. Autism Res. 2009;2:06.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Anthoni H, Sucheston-Campbell L, Lewis B, Tapia-Páez I, Fan X, Zucchelli M, Taipale M, Stein C, Hokkanen M-E, Castrén E, Pennington B, Smith S, Olson R, Tomblin J, Schulte-Körne G, Nöthen M, Schumacher J, Müller-Myhsok B, Hoffmann P, Kere J. The aromatase gene cyp19a1: several genetic and functional lines of evidence supporting a role in reading, speech and language. Behav Genet. 2012;42:509–27.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Anthoni H, Sucheston-Campbell L, Lewis B, Tapia-Páez I, Fan X, Zucchelli M, Taipale M, Stein C, Hokkanen M-E, Castrén E, Pennington B, Smith S, Olson R, Tomblin J, Schulte-Körne G, Nöthen M, Schumacher J, Müller-Myhsok B, Hoffmann P, Kere J. The aromatase gene cyp19a1: several genetic and functional lines of evidence supporting a role in reading, speech and language. Behav Genet. 2012;42:509–27.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Zettergren A, Jonsson L, Hovey D, Melke J, Lundström S, Anckarsäter H, Lichtenstein P, Westberg L. Associations between polymorphisms in sex steroid related genes and autistic-like traits. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:07.CrossRef Zettergren A, Jonsson L, Hovey D, Melke J, Lundström S, Anckarsäter H, Lichtenstein P, Westberg L. Associations between polymorphisms in sex steroid related genes and autistic-like traits. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:07.CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Zettergren A, Karlsson S, Hovey D, Jonsson L, Melke J, Anckarsäter H, Lichtenstein P, Lundström S, Westberg L. Further investigations of the relation between polymorphisms in sex steroid related genes and autistic-like trait. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;68:02.CrossRef Zettergren A, Karlsson S, Hovey D, Jonsson L, Melke J, Anckarsäter H, Lichtenstein P, Lundström S, Westberg L. Further investigations of the relation between polymorphisms in sex steroid related genes and autistic-like trait. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;68:02.CrossRef
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Selby C. Sex hormone binding globulin: origin, function and clinical significance. Ann Clin Biochem. 1990;27(Pt 6):532–41.PubMedCrossRef Selby C. Sex hormone binding globulin: origin, function and clinical significance. Ann Clin Biochem. 1990;27(Pt 6):532–41.PubMedCrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Gökçen C, Erbagci A, Mutluer T, Orkmez M, Correll C. Mullerian inhibiting substance, sex hormone binding globulin and sex hormone levels in stimulant-Naïve, first-diagnosed prepubertal boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: comparison with matched healthy controls as well as before and after oros-methylpenidate treatment. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2019;23:1–7.CrossRef Gökçen C, Erbagci A, Mutluer T, Orkmez M, Correll C. Mullerian inhibiting substance, sex hormone binding globulin and sex hormone levels in stimulant-Naïve, first-diagnosed prepubertal boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: comparison with matched healthy controls as well as before and after oros-methylpenidate treatment. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2019;23:1–7.CrossRef
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Zhao H, Nyholt D. Gene-based analyses reveal novel genetic overlap and allelic heterogeneity across five major psychiatric disorders. Hum Genet. 2017;136:02.CrossRef Zhao H, Nyholt D. Gene-based analyses reveal novel genetic overlap and allelic heterogeneity across five major psychiatric disorders. Hum Genet. 2017;136:02.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Doherty J, Owen M. Genomic insights into the overlap between psychiatric disorders: implications for research and clinical practice. Genome Med. 2014;6:29.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Doherty J, Owen M. Genomic insights into the overlap between psychiatric disorders: implications for research and clinical practice. Genome Med. 2014;6:29.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat SPARK Consortium. Spark: a u.s. cohort of 50,000 families to accelerate autism research. Neuron. 2018;97(3):488–93.CrossRef SPARK Consortium. Spark: a u.s. cohort of 50,000 families to accelerate autism research. Neuron. 2018;97(3):488–93.CrossRef
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Rutter M, Bailey A, Lord C. The social communication questionnaire: manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003. Rutter M, Bailey A, Lord C. The social communication questionnaire: manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003.
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris P, Taylor R, Minor B, Elliott V, Fernandez M, O’Neal L, McLeod L, Delacqua G, Delacqua F, Brothier J, Duda S. The redcap consortium: Building an international community of software platform partners. J Biomed Inform. 2019;95:103208.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Harris P, Taylor R, Minor B, Elliott V, Fernandez M, O’Neal L, McLeod L, Delacqua G, Delacqua F, Brothier J, Duda S. The redcap consortium: Building an international community of software platform partners. J Biomed Inform. 2019;95:103208.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Harris P, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde J. A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42:337–81.CrossRef Harris P, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde J. A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42:337–81.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Rueden C, Schindelin J, Hiner M, Dezonia B, Walter A, Eliceiri K. Imagej2: Imagej for the next generation of scientific image data. BMC Bioinform. 2017;18:01.CrossRef Rueden C, Schindelin J, Hiner M, Dezonia B, Walter A, Eliceiri K. Imagej2: Imagej for the next generation of scientific image data. BMC Bioinform. 2017;18:01.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat King D. Dlib-ml: a machine learning toolkit. J Mach Learn Res. 2009;10:1755–8. King D. Dlib-ml: a machine learning toolkit. J Mach Learn Res. 2009;10:1755–8.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Obafemi-Ajayi T, Miles J, Takahashi T, Qi W, Aldridge K, Minqi Z, Xin S-Q, He Y, Duan Y. Facial structure analysis separates autism spectrum disorders into meaningful clinical subgroups. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;45:10. Obafemi-Ajayi T, Miles J, Takahashi T, Qi W, Aldridge K, Minqi Z, Xin S-Q, He Y, Duan Y. Facial structure analysis separates autism spectrum disorders into meaningful clinical subgroups. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;45:10.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Liaw A, Wiener M. Classification and regression by random forest. R News. 2002;2(3):18–22. Liaw A, Wiener M. Classification and regression by random forest. R News. 2002;2(3):18–22.
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira M, Bender D, Maller J, Sklar P, Bakker P, Daly M, Sham P. Plink: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:559–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira M, Bender D, Maller J, Sklar P, Bakker P, Daly M, Sham P. Plink: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:559–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Ross I, Robert G. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computing. J Comput Graph Stat. 2008;5:299–314. Ross I, Robert G. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computing. J Comput Graph Stat. 2008;5:299–314.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Marees AT, de Kluiver H, Stringer S, Vorspan F, Curis E, Marie-Claire C, Derks EM. A tutorial on conducting genome-wide association studies: quality control and statistical analysis. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2018;27:e1608.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Marees AT, de Kluiver H, Stringer S, Vorspan F, Curis E, Marie-Claire C, Derks EM. A tutorial on conducting genome-wide association studies: quality control and statistical analysis. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2018;27:e1608.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
57.
Zurück zum Zitat The 1000 Genomes Project Consortium. A global reference for human genetic variation. Nature. 2015;526:68–74.CrossRef The 1000 Genomes Project Consortium. A global reference for human genetic variation. Nature. 2015;526:68–74.CrossRef
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Perreault L-P, Legault M-A, Asselin G, Dubé M-P. Genipe: an automated genome-wide imputation pipeline with automatic reporting and statistical tools. Bioinformatics. 2016;32:btw487. Perreault L-P, Legault M-A, Asselin G, Dubé M-P. Genipe: an automated genome-wide imputation pipeline with automatic reporting and statistical tools. Bioinformatics. 2016;32:btw487.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Delaneau O, Marchini J, Zagury J-F. A linear complexity phasing method for thousands of genomes. Nat Methods. 2011;9:179–81.PubMedCrossRef Delaneau O, Marchini J, Zagury J-F. A linear complexity phasing method for thousands of genomes. Nat Methods. 2011;9:179–81.PubMedCrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Howie B, Donnelly P, Marchini J. A flexible and accurate genotype imputation method for the next generation of genome-wide association studies. PLoS Genet. 2009;5:e1000529.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Howie B, Donnelly P, Marchini J. A flexible and accurate genotype imputation method for the next generation of genome-wide association studies. PLoS Genet. 2009;5:e1000529.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Feliciano P, Zhou X, Astrovskaya I, Turner TN, Wang T, Brueggeman L, Barnard R, Hsieh A, Snyder LG, Muzny DM, Sabo A, Gibbs RA, Eichler EE, O’Roak BJ, Michaelson JJ, Volfovsky N, Shen Y, Chung WK. Exome sequencing of 457 autism families recruited online provides evidence for autism risk genes. npj Genom Med. 2019;4:1–14.CrossRef Feliciano P, Zhou X, Astrovskaya I, Turner TN, Wang T, Brueggeman L, Barnard R, Hsieh A, Snyder LG, Muzny DM, Sabo A, Gibbs RA, Eichler EE, O’Roak BJ, Michaelson JJ, Volfovsky N, Shen Y, Chung WK. Exome sequencing of 457 autism families recruited online provides evidence for autism risk genes. npj Genom Med. 2019;4:1–14.CrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Choi SWS, Mak T, O’Reilly P. Tutorial: a guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nat Protoc. 2020;15:07.CrossRef Choi SWS, Mak T, O’Reilly P. Tutorial: a guide to performing polygenic risk score analyses. Nat Protoc. 2020;15:07.CrossRef
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Peila R, Arthur R, Rohan T. Association of sex hormones with risk of cancers of the pancreas, kidney and brain in the uk biobank cohort study. Cancer Epidemiol Biomark Prev. 2020;29:1832–6.CrossRef Peila R, Arthur R, Rohan T. Association of sex hormones with risk of cancers of the pancreas, kidney and brain in the uk biobank cohort study. Cancer Epidemiol Biomark Prev. 2020;29:1832–6.CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruth K, Day F, Tyrrell J, Thompson D, Wood A, Mahajan A, Beaumont R, Wittemans L, Martin S, Busch A, Erzurumluoglu AM, Hollis B, O’Mara T, McCarthy M, Langenberg C, Easton D, Wareham N, Burgess S, Murray A, Perry J. Using human genetics to understand the disease impacts of testosterone in men and women. Nat Med. 2020;26:1–7.CrossRef Ruth K, Day F, Tyrrell J, Thompson D, Wood A, Mahajan A, Beaumont R, Wittemans L, Martin S, Busch A, Erzurumluoglu AM, Hollis B, O’Mara T, McCarthy M, Langenberg C, Easton D, Wareham N, Burgess S, Murray A, Perry J. Using human genetics to understand the disease impacts of testosterone in men and women. Nat Med. 2020;26:1–7.CrossRef
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Sullivan P, Agrawal A, Bulik C, Andreassen O, BØrglum A, Breen G, Cichon S, Edenberg H, Faraone S, Gelernter J, Mathews C, Nievergelt C, Smoller J, O’Donovan M. Psychiatric genomics: an update and an agenda. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;175:15–27.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Sullivan P, Agrawal A, Bulik C, Andreassen O, BØrglum A, Breen G, Cichon S, Edenberg H, Faraone S, Gelernter J, Mathews C, Nievergelt C, Smoller J, O’Donovan M. Psychiatric genomics: an update and an agenda. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;175:15–27.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Martin J, Lee S, Mortensen P, Nordentoft M, Wray N, Franke B, Faraone S, O’Donovan M, Thapar A, Neale B, Steinhausen H-C, Walitza S. A genetic investigation of sex bias in the prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83:01.CrossRef Martin J, Lee S, Mortensen P, Nordentoft M, Wray N, Franke B, Faraone S, O’Donovan M, Thapar A, Neale B, Steinhausen H-C, Walitza S. A genetic investigation of sex bias in the prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83:01.CrossRef
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Autism Spectrum Disorders Working Group of The Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Meta-analysis of gwas of over 16,000 individuals with autism spectrum disorder highlights a novel locus at 10q24.32 and a significant overlap with schizophrenia; 2017. Autism Spectrum Disorders Working Group of The Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Meta-analysis of gwas of over 16,000 individuals with autism spectrum disorder highlights a novel locus at 10q24.32 and a significant overlap with schizophrenia; 2017.
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee J, Wedow R, Okbay A, Kong E, Maghzian O, Zacher M, Nguyen-Viet T, Bowers P, Sidorenko J, Linnér R, Fontana M, Kundu T, Lee C, Li H, Li R, Royer R, Timshel P, Walters R, Willoughby E, Cesarini D. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat Genet. 2018;50:08.CrossRef Lee J, Wedow R, Okbay A, Kong E, Maghzian O, Zacher M, Nguyen-Viet T, Bowers P, Sidorenko J, Linnér R, Fontana M, Kundu T, Lee C, Li H, Li R, Royer R, Timshel P, Walters R, Willoughby E, Cesarini D. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat Genet. 2018;50:08.CrossRef
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Euesden J, Lewis C, O’Reilly P. Prsice: polygenic risk score software. Bioinformatics. 2014;31:12. Euesden J, Lewis C, O’Reilly P. Prsice: polygenic risk score software. Bioinformatics. 2014;31:12.
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Teatero M, Netley C. A critical review of the research on the extreme male brain theory and digit ratio (2d:4d). J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43:04.CrossRef Teatero M, Netley C. A critical review of the research on the extreme male brain theory and digit ratio (2d:4d). J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43:04.CrossRef
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Hönekopp J. Digit ratio 2d:4d in relation to autism spectrum disorders, empathizing, and systemizing: a quantitative review. Autism Res Off J Int Soc Autism Res. 2012;5:221–30.CrossRef Hönekopp J. Digit ratio 2d:4d in relation to autism spectrum disorders, empathizing, and systemizing: a quantitative review. Autism Res Off J Int Soc Autism Res. 2012;5:221–30.CrossRef
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Manning J. Sex differences and age changes in digit ratios: implications for the use of digit ratios in medicine and biology. New York: Springer; 2012. p. 841–51. Manning J. Sex differences and age changes in digit ratios: implications for the use of digit ratios in medicine and biology. New York: Springer; 2012. p. 841–51.
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Kralik M, Ingrova P, Kozieł S, Hupková A, Klíma O. Overall trends vs. individual trajectories in the second-to-fourth digit (2d:4d) and metacarpal (2m:4m) ratios during puberty and adolescence. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2017;162:641–56.PubMedCrossRef Kralik M, Ingrova P, Kozieł S, Hupková A, Klíma O. Overall trends vs. individual trajectories in the second-to-fourth digit (2d:4d) and metacarpal (2m:4m) ratios during puberty and adolescence. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2017;162:641–56.PubMedCrossRef
75.
Zurück zum Zitat Francis-West P, Robson L, Evans D. Craniofacial development: the tissue and molecular interactions that control development of the head. Adv Anat Embryol Cell Biold. 2003;169:III–VI. Francis-West P, Robson L, Evans D. Craniofacial development: the tissue and molecular interactions that control development of the head. Adv Anat Embryol Cell Biold. 2003;169:III–VI.
76.
Zurück zum Zitat DeMyer W, Zeman W, Palmer C. The face predicts the brain: diagnostic significance of median facial anomalies for holoprosencephaly (arhinencephaly). Pediatrics. 1964;34:256–63.PubMedCrossRef DeMyer W, Zeman W, Palmer C. The face predicts the brain: diagnostic significance of median facial anomalies for holoprosencephaly (arhinencephaly). Pediatrics. 1964;34:256–63.PubMedCrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Diewert V, Lozanoff S, Choy V. Computer reconstruction of human craniofacial morphology showing changes in relations between the face and brain during primary palate formation. J Craniofac Genet Dev Biol. 1993;13:193–201.PubMed Diewert V, Lozanoff S, Choy V. Computer reconstruction of human craniofacial morphology showing changes in relations between the face and brain during primary palate formation. J Craniofac Genet Dev Biol. 1993;13:193–201.PubMed
78.
Zurück zum Zitat Hennessy R, McLearie S, Kinsella A, Waddington J. Facial shape and asymmetry by three-dimensional laser surface scanning covary with cognition in a sexually dimorphic manner. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;18:73–80.PubMedCrossRef Hennessy R, McLearie S, Kinsella A, Waddington J. Facial shape and asymmetry by three-dimensional laser surface scanning covary with cognition in a sexually dimorphic manner. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;18:73–80.PubMedCrossRef
79.
Zurück zum Zitat Uekermann J, Kraemer M, Abdel-Hamid M, Schimmelmann B, Hebebrand J, Daum I, Wiltfang J, Kis B. Social cognition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (adhd). Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;34:734–43.PubMedCrossRef Uekermann J, Kraemer M, Abdel-Hamid M, Schimmelmann B, Hebebrand J, Daum I, Wiltfang J, Kis B. Social cognition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (adhd). Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;34:734–43.PubMedCrossRef
80.
Zurück zum Zitat Marton K, Abramoff B, Rosenzweig S. Social cognition and language in children with specific language impairment (sli). J Commun Disord. 2005;38:143–62.PubMedCrossRef Marton K, Abramoff B, Rosenzweig S. Social cognition and language in children with specific language impairment (sli). J Commun Disord. 2005;38:143–62.PubMedCrossRef
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Bos P, Panksepp J, Bluthé R-M, van Honk J. Acute effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social-emotional behavior: a review of single administration studies. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2011;33:17–35.PubMedCrossRef Bos P, Panksepp J, Bluthé R-M, van Honk J. Acute effects of steroid hormones and neuropeptides on human social-emotional behavior: a review of single administration studies. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2011;33:17–35.PubMedCrossRef
82.
Zurück zum Zitat Skuse D, Gallagher L. Dopaminergic-neuropeptide interactions in the social brain. Trends Cognit Sci. 2009;13:27–35.CrossRef Skuse D, Gallagher L. Dopaminergic-neuropeptide interactions in the social brain. Trends Cognit Sci. 2009;13:27–35.CrossRef
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Pfaff D, Rapin I, Goldman S. Male predominance in autism: neuroendocrine influences on arousal and social anxiety. Autism Res Off J Int Soc Autism Res. 2011;4:163–76.CrossRef Pfaff D, Rapin I, Goldman S. Male predominance in autism: neuroendocrine influences on arousal and social anxiety. Autism Res Off J Int Soc Autism Res. 2011;4:163–76.CrossRef
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Bodo C, Rissman E. The androgen receptor is selectively involved in organization of sexually dimorphic social behaviors in mice. Endocrinology. 2008;149:4142–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bodo C, Rissman E. The androgen receptor is selectively involved in organization of sexually dimorphic social behaviors in mice. Endocrinology. 2008;149:4142–50.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
85.
Zurück zum Zitat Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K. Foetal testosterone, social relationships, and restricted interests in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2005;46:198–210.CrossRef Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K. Foetal testosterone, social relationships, and restricted interests in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 2005;46:198–210.CrossRef
86.
Zurück zum Zitat Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and empathy. Horm Behav. 2006;49:282–92.PubMedCrossRef Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Raggatt P, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and empathy. Horm Behav. 2006;49:282–92.PubMedCrossRef
87.
Zurück zum Zitat Lu Q, Lai J, Du Y, Huang T, Prukpitikul P, Xu Y, Hu S. Sexual dimorphism of oxytocin and vasopressin in social cognition and behavior. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2019;12:337–49.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Lu Q, Lai J, Du Y, Huang T, Prukpitikul P, Xu Y, Hu S. Sexual dimorphism of oxytocin and vasopressin in social cognition and behavior. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2019;12:337–49.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
88.
Zurück zum Zitat Baribeau D, Anagnostou E. Oxytocin and vasopressin: linking pituitary neuropeptides and their receptors to social neurocircuits. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:09.CrossRef Baribeau D, Anagnostou E. Oxytocin and vasopressin: linking pituitary neuropeptides and their receptors to social neurocircuits. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:09.CrossRef
89.
Zurück zum Zitat Parker K, Oztan O, Libove R, Mohsin N, Karhson D, Sumiyoshi R, Summers J, Hinman K, Motonaga K, Phillips J, Carson D, Fung L, Garner J, Hardan A. A randomized placebo-controlled pilot trial shows that intranasal vasopressin improves social deficits in children with autism. Sci Transl Med. 2019;11:eaau7356.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Parker K, Oztan O, Libove R, Mohsin N, Karhson D, Sumiyoshi R, Summers J, Hinman K, Motonaga K, Phillips J, Carson D, Fung L, Garner J, Hardan A. A randomized placebo-controlled pilot trial shows that intranasal vasopressin improves social deficits in children with autism. Sci Transl Med. 2019;11:eaau7356.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
90.
Zurück zum Zitat Nadler A, Jiao P, Johnson C, Alexander V, Zak P. The bull of wall street: experimental analysis of testosterone and asset trading. Manag Sci. 2018;64:4032–51.CrossRef Nadler A, Jiao P, Johnson C, Alexander V, Zak P. The bull of wall street: experimental analysis of testosterone and asset trading. Manag Sci. 2018;64:4032–51.CrossRef
91.
Zurück zum Zitat Carré J, Geniole S, Ortiz T, Bird B, Videto A, Bonin P. Exogenous testosterone rapidly increases aggressive behavior in dominant and impulsive men. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;82:06. Carré J, Geniole S, Ortiz T, Bird B, Videto A, Bonin P. Exogenous testosterone rapidly increases aggressive behavior in dominant and impulsive men. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;82:06.
92.
Zurück zum Zitat Nadler A, Zava D, Ortiz T, Watson N, Carré J, Camerer C, Nave G. Does testosterone impair mens’ cognitive empathy? Evidence from two large-scale randomized controlled trials. Proc R Soc B. 2019;286:20191062.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral Nadler A, Zava D, Ortiz T, Watson N, Carré J, Camerer C, Nave G. Does testosterone impair mens’ cognitive empathy? Evidence from two large-scale randomized controlled trials. Proc R Soc B. 2019;286:20191062.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentral
93.
Zurück zum Zitat Kranz G, Wadsak W, Kaufmann U, Savli M, Baldinger P-M, Gryglewski G, Haeusler D, Spies M, Mitterhauser M, Kasper S, Lanzenberger R. High-dose testosterone treatment increases serotonin transporter binding in transgender people. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;74:09. Kranz G, Wadsak W, Kaufmann U, Savli M, Baldinger P-M, Gryglewski G, Haeusler D, Spies M, Mitterhauser M, Kasper S, Lanzenberger R. High-dose testosterone treatment increases serotonin transporter binding in transgender people. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;74:09.
Metadaten
Titel
Genetic and morphological estimates of androgen exposure predict social deficits in multiple neurodevelopmental disorder cohorts
verfasst von
Brooke G. McKenna
Yongchao Huang
Kévin Vervier
Dabney Hofammann
Mary Cafferata
Seima Al-Momani
Florencia Lowenthal
Angela Zhang
Jin-Young Koh
Savantha Thenuwara
Leo Brueggeman
Ethan Bahl
Tanner Koomar
Natalie Pottschmidt
Taylor Kalmus
Lucas Casten
Taylor R. Thomas
Jacob J. Michaelson
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2021
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Molecular Autism / Ausgabe 1/2021
Elektronische ISSN: 2040-2392
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00450-w

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2021

Molecular Autism 1/2021 Zur Ausgabe

Neu in den Fachgebieten Neurologie und Psychiatrie

Blutdrucksenkung schon im Rettungswagen bei akutem Schlaganfall?

31.05.2024 Apoplex Nachrichten

Der optimale Ansatz für die Blutdruckkontrolle bei Patientinnen und Patienten mit akutem Schlaganfall ist noch nicht gefunden. Ob sich eine frühzeitige Therapie der Hypertonie noch während des Transports in die Klinik lohnt, hat jetzt eine Studie aus China untersucht.

Nicht Creutzfeldt Jakob, sondern Abführtee-Vergiftung

29.05.2024 Hyponatriämie Nachrichten

Eine ältere Frau trinkt regelmäßig Sennesblättertee gegen ihre Verstopfung. Der scheint plötzlich gut zu wirken. Auf Durchfall und Erbrechen folgt allerdings eine Hyponatriämie. Nach deren Korrektur kommt es plötzlich zu progredienten Kognitions- und Verhaltensstörungen.

Schutz der Synapsen bei Alzheimer

29.05.2024 Morbus Alzheimer Nachrichten

Mit einem Neurotrophin-Rezeptor-Modulator lässt sich möglicherweise eine bestehende Alzheimerdemenz etwas abschwächen: Erste Phase-2-Daten deuten auf einen verbesserten Synapsenschutz.

Hörschwäche erhöht Demenzrisiko unabhängig von Beta-Amyloid

29.05.2024 Hörstörungen Nachrichten

Hört jemand im Alter schlecht, nimmt das Hirn- und Hippocampusvolumen besonders schnell ab, was auch mit einem beschleunigten kognitiven Abbau einhergeht. Und diese Prozesse scheinen sich unabhängig von der Amyloidablagerung zu ereignen.